Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] W. Weibull. A Statistical Distribution Function of Wide Applicability , 1951 .
[2] H. Blum. A New Model of Global Brain Function , 2015, Perspectives in biology and medicine.
[3] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[4] L. Glass. Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.
[5] P. O. Bishop,et al. Spatial vision. , 1971, Annual review of psychology.
[6] R. Pérez,et al. Perception of Random Dot Interference Patterns , 1973, Nature.
[7] H. Blum. Biological shape and visual science (part I) , 1973 .
[8] H. Blum. Biological shape and visual science. I. , 1973, Journal of theoretical biology.
[9] J. Stone,et al. The naso‐temporal division of the monkey's retina , 1973, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[10] Quick Rf. A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. , 1974 .
[11] L. Glass,et al. Pattern Recognition in Humans: Correlations Which Cannot be Perceived , 1976, Perception.
[12] J. Movshon,et al. Receptive field organization of complex cells in the cat's striate cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.
[13] H. Wilson,et al. Spatial frequency tuning of orientation selective units estimated by oblique masking , 1983, Vision Research.
[14] K Prazdny,et al. On the Perception of Glass Patterns , 1984, Perception.
[15] Irving Biederman,et al. Human image understanding: Recent research and a theory , 1985, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..
[16] R. Desimone,et al. Visual properties of neurons in area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.
[17] Allen Brookes,et al. Detecting structure by symbolic constructions on tokens , 1987, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..
[18] H. Barlow,et al. Limit to the detection of Glass patterns in the presence of noise. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[19] G. Sperling,et al. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[20] N. Graham. Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .
[21] Y. Fukuda,et al. Nasotemporal overlap of crossed and uncrossed retinal ganglion cell projections in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata) , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[22] G Sperling,et al. Two motion perception mechanisms revealed through distance-driven reversal of apparent motion. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[23] D. Ts'o,et al. Functional organization of primate visual cortex revealed by high resolution optical imaging. , 1990, Science.
[24] P Perona,et al. Preattentive texture discrimination with early vision mechanisms. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[25] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Visual topography of area TEO in the macaque , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[26] Michael S. Landy,et al. Computational models of visual processing , 1991 .
[27] J. Bergen,et al. Computational Modeling of Visual Texture Segregation , 1991 .
[28] Michael S. Landy,et al. Complex Channels, Early Local Nonlinearities, and Normalization in Texture Segregation , 1991 .
[29] Lynn A. Olzak,et al. Configural effects constrain fourier models of pattern discrimination , 1992, Vision Research.
[30] Malcolm P. Young,et al. Objective analysis of the topological organization of the primate cortical visual system , 1992, Nature.
[31] A. Cowey,et al. Cortical area V4 and its role in the perception of color , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[32] H. Wilson,et al. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[33] Norma Graham,et al. Nonlinear processes in spatial-frequency channel models of perceived texture segregation: Effects of sign and amount of contrast , 1992, Vision Research.
[34] D C Van Essen,et al. Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective. , 1992, Science.
[35] H R Wilson,et al. Curvature and separation discrimination at texture boundaries. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[36] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.
[37] I Kovács,et al. A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[38] D. V. van Essen,et al. Selectivity for polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Science.
[39] H R Wilson,et al. A model for motion coherence and transparency , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.
[40] B. Julesz,et al. Perceptual sensitivity maps within globally defined visual shapes , 1994, Nature.
[41] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] D. Burr,et al. Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.
[43] W. Merigan,et al. Basic visual capacities and shape discrimination after lesions of extrastriate area V4 in macaques , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.
[44] D. C. Essen,et al. Neural responses to polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in area V4 of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[45] G. Westheimer,et al. Human Discrimination of the Implicit Orientation of Simple Symmetrical Patterns , 1997, Vision Research.
[46] H. Wilson,et al. Concentric orientation summation in human form vision , 1997, Vision Research.