The lawful imprecision of human surface tilt estimation in natural scenes
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Appelle. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.
[2] Jeanny Hérault,et al. Model of Frequency Analysis in the Visual Cortex and the Shape from Texture Problem , 2008, International Journal of Computer Vision.
[3] M. Hayhoe,et al. In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? , 2001, Vision Research.
[4] Stéphane Mallat,et al. The Texture Gradient Equation for Recovering Shape from Texture , 2002, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[5] Johannes Burge,et al. Accuracy Maximization Analysis for Sensory-Perceptual Tasks: Computational Improvements, Filter Robustness, and Coding Advantages for Scaled Additive Noise , 2017, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[6] Hideko F. Norman,et al. Visual discrimination of local surface structure: Slant, tilt, and curvedness , 2006, Vision Research.
[7] Dale Purves,et al. Image/source statistics of surfaces in natural scenes , 2003, Network.
[8] M. Landy,et al. Why Is Spatial Stereoresolution So Low? , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] R. F. Wagner,et al. Efficiency of human visual signal discrimination. , 1981, Science.
[10] James H. Elder,et al. Texture properties affecting the accuracy of surface attitude judgements , 2006, Vision Research.
[11] Thomas Martinetz,et al. Variability of eye movements when viewing dynamic natural scenes. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[12] Dale Purves,et al. A statistical explanation of visual space , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[13] Johannes Burge,et al. Linking normative models of natural tasks to descriptive models of neural response , 2017, bioRxiv.
[14] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Optimal speed estimation in natural image movies predicts human performance , 2015, Nature Communications.
[15] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Optimal defocus estimates from individual images for autofocusing a digital camera , 2012, Electronic Imaging.
[16] Jitendra Malik,et al. Surface orientation from texture: Isotropy or homogeneity (or both)? , 1997, Vision Research.
[17] H. Bülthoff,et al. 3D shape perception from combined depth cues in human visual cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[18] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[19] Christopher W. Tyler,et al. Binocular cross-correlation in time and space , 1978, Vision Research.
[20] Qasim Zaidi,et al. Three-dimensional shape from non-homogeneous textures: carved and stretched surfaces. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[21] W. Geisler,et al. Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[22] James M. Hillis,et al. Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[23] S. McKee,et al. Disparity increment thresholds for gratings. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[24] Kent A. Stevens,et al. Slant-tilt: The visual encoding of surface orientation , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.
[25] Johannes Burge,et al. Optimal defocus estimation in individual natural images , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] Jiri Najemnik,et al. Optimal stimulus encoders for natural tasks. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[27] M. Landy,et al. Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error , 2003, Vision Research.
[28] Takahisa M. Sanada,et al. Representation of 3-D surface orientation by velocity and disparity gradient cues in area MT. , 2012, Journal of Neurophysiology.
[29] Adam Binch,et al. Perception as Bayesian Inference , 2014 .
[30] H. Sakata,et al. Integration of perspective and disparity cues in surface-orientation-selective neurons of area CIP. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[31] M. Banks,et al. Visual–Haptic Adaptation Is Determined by Relative Reliability , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[32] H. Bülthoff,et al. Estimation of 3D shape from image orientations , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[33] M. Ernst,et al. Focus cues affect perceived depth. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[34] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] J. Pelz,et al. Oculomotor behavior in natural and man-made environments , 2007 .
[36] Charless C. Fowlkes,et al. Natural-Scene Statistics Predict How the Figure–Ground Cue of Convexity Affects Human Depth Perception , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] John P. Wann,et al. Where you look when you learn to steer , 2004 .
[38] Pascal Mamassian,et al. Temporal dynamics in bistable perception. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[39] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] J. Todd,et al. Effects of changing viewing conditions on the perceived structure of smoothly curved surfaces. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[42] Julian Leyland,et al. The Southampton-York Natural Scenes (SYNS) dataset: Statistics of surface attitude , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[43] C. Furmanski,et al. An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[44] Jitendra Malik,et al. Computing Local Surface Orientation and Shape from Texture for Curved Surfaces , 1997, International Journal of Computer Vision.
[45] J. Todd. Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.3 March 2004 The visual perception of 3D shape q , 2022 .
[46] Barton L. Anderson,et al. Coupled computations of three-dimensional shape and material , 2015, Current Biology.
[47] Ari Rosenberg,et al. The Visual Representation of 3D Object Orientation in Parietal Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[48] Robin L. Hill,et al. Eye movements : a window on mind and brain , 2007 .
[49] Andrea Li,et al. Perception of three-dimensional shape from texture is based on patterns of oriented energy , 2000, Vision Research.
[50] Josh H. McDermott,et al. Psychophysics with junctions in real images. , 2010, Perception.
[51] David C. Knill,et al. Surface orientation from texture: ideal observers, generic observers and the information content of texture cues , 1998, Vision Research.
[52] H. Barlow. Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information: David Marr. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1982. pp. xvi + 397 , 1983 .
[53] Joan Lasenby,et al. Shape from Texture: Fast Estimation of Planar Surface Orientation via Fourier Analysis , 2007, BMVC.
[54] J. Saunders,et al. Perception of 3D surface orientation from skew symmetry , 2001, Vision Research.
[55] James T Todd,et al. The visual perception of 3-D shape from multiple cues: Are observers capable of perceiving metric structure? , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[56] Brian C. McCann,et al. Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes , 2016, Journal of vision.
[57] D. Knill. Ideal observer perturbation analysis reveals human strategies for inferring surface orientation from texture , 1998, Vision Research.
[58] Hiroshi Ban,et al. Integration of texture and disparity cues to surface slant in dorsal visual cortex. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.