fMRI Activation in a Visual-Perception Task: Network of Areas Detected Using the General Linear Model and Independent Components Analysis

The Motor-Free Visual Perception Test, revised (MVPT-R), provides a measure of visual perceptual processing. It involves different cognitive elements including visual discrimination, spatial relationships, and mental rotation. We adapted the MVPT-R to an event-related functional MRI (fMRI) environment to investigate the brain regions involved in the interrelation of these cognitive elements. Two complementary analysis methods were employed to characterize the fMRI data: (a) a general linear model SPM approach based upon a model of the time course and a hemodynamic response estimate and (b) independent component analysis (ICA), which does not constrain the specific shape of the time course per se, although we did require it to be at least transiently task-related. Additionally, we implemented ICA in a novel way to create a group average that was compared with the SPM group results. Both methods yielded similar, but not identical, results and detected a network of robustly activated visual, inferior parietal, and frontal eye-field areas as well as thalamus and cerebellum. SPM appeared to be the more sensitive method and has a well-developed theoretical approach to thresholding. The ICA method segregated functional elements into separate maps and identified additional regions with extended activation in response to presented events. The results demonstrate the utility of complementary analyses for fMRI data and suggest that the cerebellum may play a significant role in visual perceptual processing. Additionally, results illustrate functional connectivity between frontal eye fields and prefrontal and parietal regions.

[1]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow and cognitive function in Huntington's disease and schizophrenia. A comparison of patients matched for performance on a prefrontal-type task. , 1990, Archives of neurology.

[2]  N. Sartorius,et al.  New versions of World Health Organization instruments for the assessment of mental disorders , 1994, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[3]  C. Connor,et al.  Covert attention suppresses neuronal responses in area 7a of the posterior parietal cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  An Information-Maximization Approach to Blind Separation and Blind Deconvolution , 1995, Neural Computation.

[5]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[7]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[9]  Peter Andersen,et al.  Quantitative relations between parietal activation and performance in mental rotation , 1996, Neuroreport.

[10]  R. Woods Modeling for Intergroup Comparisons of Imaging Data , 1996, NeuroImage.

[11]  J. Bloedel,et al.  Duality of cerebellar motor and cognitive functions. , 1997, International review of neurobiology.

[12]  D Le Bihan,et al.  Latencies in fMRI time‐series: effect of slice acquisition order and perception , 1997, NMR in biomedicine.

[13]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[14]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[15]  S Makeig,et al.  Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[16]  Toshio Inui,et al.  The role of the posterior parietal cortex in human object recognition: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.

[18]  T. Raij Patterns of Brain Activity during Visual Imagery of Letters , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  L. K. Hansen,et al.  Plurality and Resemblance in fMRI Data Analysis , 1999, NeuroImage.

[20]  M. Honda,et al.  Attentional modulation of parieto-occipital cortical responses: implications for hemispatial neglect , 2000, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[21]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades , 2000, Neuroreport.

[22]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[23]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  T. Nakada,et al.  Independent component-cross correlation-sequential epoch (ICS) analysis of high field fMRI time series: direct visualization of dual representation of the primary motor cortex in human , 2000, Neuroscience Research.

[25]  Comparing activation of a cerebro-cerebellar network during saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements. An fMRI study , 2000, NeuroImage.

[26]  V D Calhoun,et al.  Spatial and temporal independent component analysis of functional MRI data containing a pair of task‐related waveforms , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[27]  T. Adali,et al.  INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS APPLIED TO FMRI DATA: A NATURAL MODEL AND ORDER SELECTION , 2001 .

[28]  J. Pekar,et al.  A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.