The Role of Animism Tendencies and Empathy in Adult Evaluations of Robot

We investigated whether Japanese adults' beliefs about friendship and morality toward robots differing in appearance (i.e., humanoid, dog-like, and egg-shaped) related to their animism tendencies and empathy. University students responded to questionnaires regarding three animism tendencies (i.e., general animism or a tendency to believe souls or gods in nonliving things, aliveness animism or a tendency to consider nonliving things as live entities, and agentic animisms or a tendency to attribute biological, artifactual, psychological, perceptual, and naming properties) and empathy. We found that friendship and morality were related to slightly different animism tendencies and empathy even though they shared some major factors. Aliveness animism, as well as a tendency to attribute perceptual and name properties toward robots, might be necessary for an individual to believe that robots could be social agents. Participants who responded that robots could be their friends showed a tendency to feel a soul in manmade objects and a strong self-oriented emotional reactivity, whereas participants who answered that robots were moral beings showed a tendency to exhibit strong emotional susceptibility. We discuss implications of these results and reasons why people feel that robots have a mind or consciousness.

[1]  The Meaning of life: Animism in the Classificatory Skills of Older Adults , 2000, International journal of aging & human development.

[2]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. , 2007, Psychological review.

[3]  Yumi Suzuki,et al.  Development of the Multidimensional Empathy Scale (MES) , 2008 .

[4]  Batya Friedman,et al.  Robotic pets in the lives of preschool children , 2004, CHI EA '04.

[5]  Shoji Itakura,et al.  Development of Mentalizing and Communication: From Viewpoint of Developmental Cybernetics and Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2008, IEICE Trans. Commun..

[6]  Brian T. Gill,et al.  "Robovie, you'll have to go into the closet now": children's social and moral relationships with a humanoid robot. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[7]  S. Gelman,et al.  Preschoolers' Ability to Distinguish Living Kinds as a Function of Regrowth , 1993 .

[8]  T. Kanda,et al.  How to Build an Intentional Android: Infants' Imitation of a Robot's Goal-Directed Actions , 2008 .

[9]  Márta Gácsi,et al.  Should we love robots? - The most liked qualities of companion dogs and how they can be implemented in social robots , 2018, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[10]  Michiteru Kitazaki,et al.  Young Children's Folk Knowledge of Robots , 2010 .

[11]  R. J. B. The Child's Conception of the World , 1929, Nature.

[12]  Children's understanding of animal, plant, and artifact properties between 3 and 6 years , 2017 .

[13]  Tatsuya Nomura,et al.  What People Assume about Humanoid and Animal-Type Robots: Cross-Cultural Analysis between Japan, Korea, and the United States , 2008, Int. J. Humanoid Robotics.

[14]  H. Ikeuchi Animistic thinking in adults : The memorial service for dolls as a voluntary loss , 2010 .

[15]  Tatsuya Nomura,et al.  The influence of people’s culture and prior experiences with Aibo on their attitude towards robots , 2006, AI & SOCIETY.

[16]  Steven O. Entezari,et al.  Individual differences predict sensitivity to the uncanny valley , 2015 .

[17]  Takayuki Kanda,et al.  Escaping from Children’s Abuse of Social Robots , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[18]  D. Wegner,et al.  Feeling robots and human zombies: Mind perception and the uncanny valley , 2012, Cognition.

[19]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Developmental “Roots” in Mature Biological Knowledge , 2009, Psychological science.

[20]  Karl F. MacDorman,et al.  Does Japan really have robot mania? Comparing attitudes by implicit and explicit measures , 2008, AI & SOCIETY.

[21]  E. B. Tylor,et al.  Primitive Culture: Researches Into the Development of Mythology, Philosophy, Religion, Art, and Custom , 1974 .

[22]  G. Hatano,et al.  Young children's recognition of commonalities between animals and plants. , 1996, Child development.

[23]  Pei-Luen Patrick Rau,et al.  A Cross-cultural Study: Effect of Robot Appearance and Task , 2010, Int. J. Soc. Robotics.

[24]  S. Gelman,et al.  Robots and rodents: children's inferences about living and nonliving kinds. , 2007, Child development.

[25]  Yuichiro Yoshikawa,et al.  Creation and Staging of Android Theatre "Sayonara"towards Developing Highly Human-Like Robots , 2017, Future Internet.

[26]  Nathan G. Freier,et al.  Social and moral relationships with robotic others? , 2004, RO-MAN 2004. 13th IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (IEEE Catalog No.04TH8759).

[27]  Batya Friedman,et al.  Robots as dogs?: children's interactions with the robotic dog AIBO and a live australian shepherd , 2005, CHI Extended Abstracts.

[28]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Revisiting preschoolers’ living things concept: A microgenetic analysis of conceptual change in basic biology , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Parent–child conversations regarding the ontological status of a robotic dog , 2016 .