Neural Basis of Adaptive Response Time Adjustment during Saccade Countermanding

Humans and macaque monkeys adjust their response time adaptively in stop-signal (countermanding) tasks, responding slower after stop-signal trials than after control trials with no stop signal. We investigated the neural mechanism underlying this adaptive response time adjustment in macaque monkeys performing a saccade countermanding task. Earlier research showed that movements are initiated when the random accumulation of presaccadic movement-related activity reaches a fixed threshold. We found that a systematic delay in response time after stop-signal trials was accomplished not through a change of threshold, baseline, or accumulation rate, but instead through a change in the time when activity first began to accumulate. The neurons underlying movement initiation have been identified with stochastic accumulator models of response time performance. Therefore, this new result provides surprising new insights into the neural instantiation of stochastic accumulator models and the mechanisms through which executive control can be exerted.

[1]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  F. Lui,et al.  Projections from visual areas of the cerebral cortex to pretectal nuclear complex, terminal accessory optic nuclei, and superior colliculus in macaque monkey , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[4]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys , 2007, Vision Research.

[5]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  A. Osman,et al.  On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  Hiroyuki Nakahara,et al.  Extended LATER model can account for trial-by-trial variability of both pre- and post-processes , 2006, Neural Networks.

[8]  KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al.  Optimal performance in a countermanding saccade task , 2010, Brain Research.

[9]  G. Woodman,et al.  The Effect of Visual Search Efficiency on Response Preparation , 2008, Psychological science.

[10]  V. Stuphorn,et al.  Supplementary Motor Area Exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Executive control of gaze by the frontal lobes , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  V. Stuphorn,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex Motivates But Does Not Control Movement Initiation in the Countermanding Task , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Neural control of visual search by frontal eye field: effects of unexpected target displacement on visual selection and saccade preparation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  R H S Carpenter,et al.  A simple two‐stage model predicts response time distributions , 2009, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[17]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Reliability of Macaque Frontal Eye Field Neurons Signaling Saccade Targets during Visual Search , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Bullier,et al.  Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Schall,et al.  Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[21]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[22]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Long-term aftereffects of response inhibition: memory retrieval, task goals, and cognitive control. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  P. Enticott,et al.  ERP correlates of response inhibition after-effects in the stop signal task , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[25]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[26]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[27]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Philip Holmes,et al.  Rapid decision threshold modulation by reward rate in a neural network , 2006, Neural Networks.

[29]  G. Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Nonstationarity of saccade response time in stopping and stepping tasks , 2008 .

[31]  G. Logan,et al.  Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Martina Rieger,et al.  Inhibitory after‐effects in the stop signal paradigm , 1999 .

[33]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[34]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. , 2010, Psychological review.

[35]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  M. Rushworth Intention, Choice, and the Medial Frontal Cortex , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Search Efficiency but Not Response Interference Affects Visual Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2001, Neuron.

[38]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[40]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[42]  C. Kennard,et al.  Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  D. Sparks Functional properties of neurons in the monkey superior colliculus: Coupling of neuronal activity and saccade onset , 1978, Brain Research.

[45]  A. Osman,et al.  Mechanisms of speed–accuracy tradeoff: evidence from covert motor processes , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[46]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Relation of frontal eye field activity to saccade initiation during a countermanding task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Jeffrey D Schall,et al.  Functional distinction between visuomovement and movement neurons in macaque frontal eye field during saccade countermanding. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[49]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[50]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[51]  P. May The mammalian superior colliculus: laminar structure and connections. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[52]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[53]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold your horses: A dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.

[54]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[55]  Veit Stuphorn,et al.  Role of supplementary eye field in saccade initiation: executive, not direct, control. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  J. Schall On building a bridge between brain and behavior. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[58]  T. Rowan Functional stability analysis of numerical algorithms , 1990 .

[59]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[60]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[61]  Chung-Chuan Lo,et al.  Proactive Inhibitory Control and Attractor Dynamics in Countermanding Action: A Spiking Neural Circuit Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.