Oculomotor inhibition reflects temporal expectations

&NA; The accurate extraction of signals out of noisy environments is a major challenge of the perceptual system. Forming temporal expectations and continuously matching them with perceptual input can facilitate this process. In humans, temporal expectations are typically assessed using behavioral measures, which provide only retrospective but no real‐time estimates during target anticipation, or by using electrophysiological measures, which require extensive preprocessing and are difficult to interpret. Here we show a new correlate of temporal expectations based on oculomotor behavior. Observers performed an orientation‐discrimination task on a central grating target, while their gaze position and EEG were monitored. In each trial, a cue preceded the target by a varying interval (“foreperiod”). In separate blocks, the cue was either predictive or non‐predictive regarding the timing of the target. Results showed that saccades and blinks were inhibited more prior to an anticipated regular target than a less‐anticipated irregular one. This consistent oculomotor inhibition effect enabled a trial‐by‐trial classification according to interval‐regularity. Additionally, in the regular condition the slope of saccade‐rate and drift were shallower for longer than shorter foreperiods, indicating their adjustment according to temporal expectations. Comparing the sensitivity of this oculomotor marker with those of other common predictability markers (e.g. alpha‐suppression) showed that it is a sensitive marker for cue‐related anticipation. In contrast, temporal changes in conditional probabilities (hazard‐rate) modulated alpha‐suppression more than cue‐related anticipation. We conclude that pre‐target oculomotor inhibition is a correlate of temporal predictions induced by cue‐target associations, whereas alpha‐suppression is more sensitive to conditional probabilities across time. HighlightsSaccades and blinks are inhibited prior to predictable targets.Pre‐target oculomotor inhibition can be used as an index of temporal expectations.This index is sensitive and direct, and is measured while predictions are made, unlike retrospective behavioral measures.EEG Alpha amplitude is correlated with the hazard rate and reflects conditional probabilities.There is no evidence that the oculomotor index reflects conditional probabilities.

[1]  A. Sereno,et al.  Levodopa slows prosaccades and improves antisaccades: an eye movement study in Parkinson’s disease , 2006, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[2]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[3]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Two distinct neural effects of blinking on human visual processing , 2005, NeuroImage.

[4]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Response force is sensitive to the temporal uncertainty of response stimuli , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Tobias Banaschewski,et al.  Cognitive and neurophysiological markers of ADHD persistence and remission , 2016, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[6]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Eishi Asano,et al.  Human occipital cortices differentially exert saccadic suppression: Intracranial recording in children , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  R. Quiroga,et al.  Human single-neuron responses at the threshold of conscious recognition , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Wim J. Riedel,et al.  Contingent negative variation as a dopaminergic biomarker: evidence from dose-related effects of methylphenidate , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[10]  U. Polat,et al.  ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication , 2014, Vision Research.

[11]  Sylvie Belleville,et al.  Age-related differences in response preparation: the role of time uncertainty. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[12]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[13]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  CNV and temporal uncertainty with ‘ageing’ and ‘non-ageing’ S1–S2 intervals , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  俊 松田,et al.  随伴陰性変動 (contingent negative variation : CNV) と有意味材料の記銘検索 , 1983 .

[15]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Word Recognition: A Systematic Comparison of Natural Reading and Rapid Serial Visual Presentation , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Acting without seeing: eye movements reveal visual processing without awareness , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  M. Morrone,et al.  Extraretinal Control of Saccadic Suppression , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Dekel Abeles,et al.  Temporal dynamics of saccades explained by a self-paced process , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[19]  B. L. Zuber,et al.  Microsaccades and the Velocity-Amplitude Relationship for Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[20]  Anne E Carpenter,et al.  Neuron-type specific signals for reward and punishment in the ventral tegmental area , 2011, Nature.

[21]  L. Deouell,et al.  Neural mechanisms of rhythm-based temporal prediction: Delta phase-locking reflects temporal predictability but not rhythmic entrainment , 2017, PLoS biology.

[22]  Martin Rolfs,et al.  Oculomotor inhibition covaries with conscious detection. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  B. L. Zuber,et al.  Saccadic suppression: elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. , 1966, Experimental neurology.

[24]  W. H. Teichner Recent studies of simple reaction time. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  A. Nobre,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Related to Anticipatory Attention Follow Temporal Expectations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  R. VanRullen,et al.  This Is the Rhythm of Your Eyes: The Phase of Ongoing Electroencephalogram Oscillations Modulates Saccadic Reaction Time , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[30]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkey during a Covert Visual Attention Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Ralph Barnes,et al.  Expectancy, Attention, and Time , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  C Miniussi,et al.  Orienting attention in time. , 2001, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[33]  W. Singer,et al.  Gamma Responses Correlate with Temporal Expectation in Monkey Primary Visual Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  David Ottoson,et al.  The Neural Basis of Perception , 1983 .

[35]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.

[36]  Reinhold Kliegl,et al.  Trans-saccadic parafoveal preview benefits in fluent reading: A study with fixation-related brain potentials , 2012, NeuroImage.

[37]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Neuronal oscillations as a mechanistic substrate of auditory temporal prediction , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[38]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Neuronal Coherence as a Mechanism of Effective Corticospinal Interaction , 2005, Science.

[39]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Microsaccadic Suppression of Visual Bursts in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Daniel Wilson,et al.  Contingent negative variation (CNV) associated with sensorimotor timing error correction , 2016, NeuroImage.

[41]  Christian N L Olivers,et al.  Oculomotor measures reveal the temporal dynamics of preparing for search. , 2017, Progress in brain research.

[42]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Posterior and anterior contribution of hand-movement preparation to late CNV. , 2000 .

[43]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  S. Martinez-Conde,et al.  The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Turatto,et al.  Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades , 2006, Neuroreport.

[46]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Resampling-based false discovery rate controlling multiple test procedures for correlated test statistics , 1999 .

[47]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[48]  Susan A. King,et al.  Simultaneous Recordings of Human Microsaccades and Drifts with a Contemporary Video Eye Tracker and the Search Coil Technique , 2015, PloS one.

[49]  Ziad M. Hafed Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades , 2013, Neuron.

[50]  D. Kourtis,et al.  Neurophysiology of Implicit Timing in Serial Choice Reaction-Time Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Prolonged Dopamine Signalling in Striatum Signals Proximity and Value of Distant Rewards , 2013, Nature.

[52]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[53]  N. Narayanan,et al.  Prefrontal D1 dopamine signaling is necessary for temporal expectation during reaction time performance , 2013, Neuroscience.

[54]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Saccades phase-locked to alpha oscillations in the occipital and medial temporal lobe enhance memory encoding , 2017, bioRxiv.

[55]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[56]  Todd M. Herrington,et al.  The Effect of Microsaccades on the Correlation between Neural Activity and Behavior in Middle Temporal, Ventral Intraparietal, and Lateral Intraparietal Areas , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Britt Anderson,et al.  Effects of temporal context and temporal expectancy on neural activity in inferior temporal cortex , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  A. Baumeister,et al.  Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency , 1969 .

[59]  D. Terhune,et al.  Time dilates after spontaneous blinking , 2016, Current Biology.

[60]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Attention and Temporal Expectations Modulate Power, Not Phase, of Ongoing Alpha Oscillations , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.

[62]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Physiological reviews.

[63]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[64]  H. Geurts,et al.  The top and the bottom of ADHD: a neuropsychological perspective , 2003, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[65]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Tracking the eye non-invasively: simultaneous comparison of the scleral search coil and optical tracking techniques in the macaque monkey , 2012, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[66]  P. Wojtkowski Temporal Dynamics , 2018, Encyclopedia of GIS.

[67]  M. García-Pérez,et al.  Forced-choice staircases with fixed step sizes: asymptotic and small-sample properties , 1998, Vision Research.

[68]  Ronen Hershman,et al.  A novel blink detection method based on pupillometry noise , 2018, Behavior Research Methods.

[69]  Niko A. Busch,et al.  Probing the dynamics of perisaccadic vision with EEG , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[71]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Prestimulus Inhibition of Saccades in Adults With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder as an Index of Temporal Expectations , 2017, Psychological science.

[72]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[73]  E. Large,et al.  The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. , 1999 .

[74]  Trevor Hastie,et al.  Neural mechanisms of rhythm-based temporal prediction : Delta phase-locking reflects temporal predictability but not rhythmic entrainment , 2017 .

[75]  Richard Ragot,et al.  Relationship between CNV and timing of an upcoming event , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.