Pain threshold variations in somatic wall tissues as a function of menstrual cycle, segmental site and tissue depth in non-dysmenorrheic women, dysmenorrheic women and men

Abstract Pain symptoms of many disorders are reported to vary with menstrual stage. This study investigated how pain thresholds to electrical stimulation of the skin, subcutis and muscle tissue varied with menstrual stage in normal women and compared these variations with those in women with dysmenorrhea and in healthy men at matched intervals. Thresholds of the three tissues were measured four times during the course of one menstrual cycle at four sites. Two of the sites were on the abdomen within the uterine viscerotome (abdomen‐rectus abdominis, left and right) and two were outside it on the limbs (leg‐quadriceps, arm‐deltoid). Calculated from the beginning of menstruation (day 0), the menstrual phases studied were menstrual (days 2–6), periovulatory (days 12–16), luteal (days 17–22) and premenstrual (days 25–28). Spontaneous pain associated with menstruation was measured from diary estimates on a VAS scale. Menstrual phase, dysmenorrhea and tissue: Whereas the highest thresholds always occurred in the luteal phase regardless of segmental site or stimulus depth, the lowest thresholds occurred in the periovulatory stage for skin, whereas those for muscle/subcutis occurred perimenstrually. Dysmenorrhea accentuated the impact of menstrual phase. For non‐dysmenorrheic women menstrual trends were significant only in abdominal muscle and subcutis, but for dysmenorrheic women the trends were also significant in abdominal skin and in limb muscle and subcutis. Dysmenorrhea also lowered thresholds mainly in muscle and sometimes in subcutis, but never in skin, with the greatest hyperalgesic effects in left abdominis muscle. Segmental site: Abdominal sites were more vulnerable to menstrual influences than limb sites. Muscle thresholds, but not skin or subcutis thresholds, were significantly lower in abdomen than in limbs, particularly in dysmenorrheic women. The amount of abdominal muscle hyperalgesia correlated significantly with the amount of spontaneous menstrual pain. Sex differences: Only minor sex differences were observed for pain thresholds of the arm and leg, but there was a unanimous refusal by men, but not by women, to be tested at abdominal sites. These results indicate that menstrual phase, dysmenorrhea status, segmental site, tissue depth and sex all have unique interacting effects on pain thresholds, thus adding more items to the lengthy and still‐growing list of biological factors that enter into an individual's judgment of whether or not a stimulus is painful.

[1]  E. Klonoff,et al.  Appraisal and response to pain may be a function of its bodily location. , 1993, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[2]  K. Hagbarth,et al.  Manoeuvres affecting sympathetic outflow in human skin nerves. , 1972, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[3]  P. Holzer,et al.  Visceral vasodilatation and somatic vasoconstriction evoked by acid challenge of the rat gastric mucosa: diversity of mechanisms. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  M. Giamberardino,et al.  Sensory characterization of somatic parietal tissues in humans with chronic fatigue syndrome , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  J. Drukker,et al.  The extrinsic innervation of the pelvic organs in the female rat. , 1980, Acta anatomica.

[6]  P. Thompson,et al.  Plasma β-endorphin, pain thresholds and anxiety levels across the human menstrual cycle , 1984, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  R. Helme,et al.  Altered heat pain thresholds and cerebral event-related potentials following painful CO2 laser stimulation in subjects with fibromyalgia syndrome , 1994, Pain.

[8]  P. Goolkasian Cyclic changes in pain perception: An ROC analysis , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  Steven F. Maier,et al.  Immune activation: the role of pro-inflammatory cytokines in inflammation, illness responses and pathological pain states , 1995, Pain.

[10]  J. Travell Myofascial pain and dysfunction , 1983 .

[11]  M. Giamberardino,et al.  Muscular hyperalgesia of renal/ureteral origin , 1993 .

[12]  K. Berkley,et al.  Estrous and sex variations in vocalization thresholds to hindpaw and tail pressure stimulation in the rat , 1996, Brain Research.

[13]  R. Halfens,et al.  [Pain in children]. , 1994, TVZ : het vakblad voor de verpleging.

[14]  D. Albe-Fessard,et al.  Pain from renal/ureteral calculosis: evaluation of sensory thresholds in the lumbar area , 1989, Pain.

[15]  F. Francini,et al.  The effects of non-painful transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation on cutaneous pain threshold and muscular reflexes in normal men and in subjects with chronic pain , 1981, Pain.

[16]  R. Herren The effect of high and low female sex hormone concentration on the two-point threshold of pain and touch and upon tactile sensitivity. , 1933 .

[17]  Weiert Velle,et al.  Sex Differences in Sensory Functions , 2015, Perspectives in biology and medicine.

[18]  R. Duranti,et al.  Pain thresholds and electromyographic features of periarticular muscles in patients with osteoarthritis of the knee , 1981, PAIN.

[19]  K J Berkley Sex differences in pain , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  M. Maresca,et al.  Clinical approach to visceral sensation. , 1986, Progress in brain research.

[21]  M. Bartelink,et al.  Changes in skin blood flow during the menstrual cycle: the influence of the menstrual cycle on the peripheral circulation in healthy female volunteers. , 1990, Clinical science.

[22]  B. Bock,et al.  Hormonal milieu affects tailflick latency in female rats and may be attenuated by access to sucrose , 1992, Physiology & Behavior.

[23]  H. E. Adams,et al.  Autonomic arousal and pain sensitivity in women with premenstrual syndrome at different phases of the menstrual cycle. , 1986, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[24]  Roger B. Fillingim,et al.  Gender differences in the responses to noxious stimuli , 1995 .

[25]  M. McCarthy Functional Significance of Steroid Modulation of GABAergic Neurotransmission: Analysis at the Behavioral, Cellular, and Molecular Levels , 1995, Hormones and Behavior.

[26]  Devin J. Starlanyl Fibromyalgia and Myofascial Pain Syndrome: A Special Challenge , 1996 .

[27]  J. Haman,et al.  Pain Threshold in Dysmenorrhea , 1944 .

[28]  P. Pacheco,et al.  Assessing pain threshold in the rat: Changes with estrus and time of day , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[29]  D. Catanzaro,et al.  Sensitivity to cold pressor pain in dysmenorrheic and non-dysmenorrheic women as a function of menstrual cycle phase , 1988, Pain.

[30]  R. M. Gold,et al.  Differential effects of ovarian hormones on reactivity to electric footshock in the rat , 1978, Physiology & Behavior.

[31]  L. Colledge Referred Pain. , 1932, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[32]  John Cp Henderson,et al.  Myofascial Pain and Dysfunction: The Trigger Point Manual , 1984 .

[33]  P. Wall,et al.  Textbook of pain , 1989 .

[34]  C. Kelleher,et al.  Blood pressure alters during the normal menstrual cycle , 1986, British journal of obstetrics and gynaecology.

[35]  R. F. Vollman The menstrual cycle. , 1977, Major problems in obstetrics and gynecology.

[36]  L. Vecchiet,et al.  Effects of extracorporeal shock-wave lithotripsy on referred hyperalgesia from renal/ureteral calculosis , 1994, Pain.

[37]  D. Denney,et al.  Pain sensitivity and coping strategies among dysmenorrheic women: much ado about nothing. , 1983, Behaviour research and therapy.

[38]  J. Perich [Menstrual cycle]. , 1950, Journal des praticiens; revue generale de clinique et de therapeutique.

[39]  P. Goolkasian Phase and Sex Effects in Pain Perception: A Critical Review , 1985 .

[40]  W. H. Tedford,et al.  Alteration of shock aversion thresholds during the menstrual cycle , 1977 .

[41]  W. Maixner,et al.  Pain sensitivity in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A preliminary report , 1995 .

[42]  P. Goolkasian An ROC analysis of pain reactions in dysmenorrheic and nondysmenorrheic women , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.