Magnetic Stimulation of Extrastriate Body Area Impairs Visual Processing of Nonfacial Body Parts
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Farah,et al. The psychological reality of the body schema: a test with normal participants. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Chi-Hung Juan,et al. Feedback to V1: a reverse hierarchy in vision , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] M. Corbetta,et al. Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] U. Mosimann,et al. Hemispheric asymmetry in visuospatial attention assessed with transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[5] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[6] N. Kanwisher,et al. The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[7] C. Guariglia,et al. Is autotopoagnosia real? EC says yes. A case study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[8] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.
[9] E. Wassermann. Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[10] C. Price,et al. A functional neuroimaging study of the variables that generate category-specific object processing differences. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al. Handbook of transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2002 .
[12] Alan Cowey,et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[13] Atsushi Yamadori,et al. Category-specific comprehension deficit restricted to body parts , 1997 .
[14] The psychological reality of the body schema: a test with normal participants. , 1995 .
[15] J. Marshall,et al. Unilateral Somatoparaphrenia After Right Hemisphere Stroke: A Case Description , 1995, Cortex.
[16] S. Aglioti,et al. The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.
[17] S. Aglioti,et al. Disownership of left hand and objects related to it in a patient with right brain damage , 1996, Neuroreport.
[18] Guy Orban,et al. The Role of Lateral Occipitotemporal Junction and Area MT/V5 in the Visual Analysis of Upper-Limb Postures , 2000, NeuroImage.
[19] V. Stone,et al. The Body-Inversion Effect , 2003, Psychological science.
[20] P. Downing,et al. Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected , 2004, Cognition.
[21] Tjeerd Jellema,et al. Cells in monkey STS responsive to articulated body motions and consequent static posture: a case of implied motion? , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[22] Doris Y. Tsao,et al. Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] A. Caramazza,et al. Domain-Specific Knowledge Systems in the Brain: The Animate-Inanimate Distinction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] Aina Puce,et al. Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[25] D. Le Bihan,et al. Distinct Cortical Areas for Names of Numbers and Body Parts Independent of Language and Input Modality , 2000, NeuroImage.
[26] A. Paans,et al. Brain Activation Related to the Representations of External Space and Body Scheme in Visuomotor Control , 2001, NeuroImage.
[27] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[28] O. Blanke,et al. Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[29] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The Selective Sparing of Body Part Knowledge: A Case Study , 1998 .
[31] G B Arden,et al. The Visual System , 2021, AMA Guides to the Evaluation of Permanent Impairment, 6th Edition, 2021.
[32] M. Petrides,et al. Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[33] Eleanor M. Saffran,et al. Knowledge of the human body: A distinct semantic domain , 2002, Neurology.
[34] J. Haxby,et al. Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] P. Downing,et al. Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] J. Barton,et al. Disorders of face perception and recognition. , 2003, Neurologic clinics.
[37] R. Nebes,et al. Patterns of Hand Preference in a Student Population , 1975, Cortex.
[38] Sarah H. Creem,et al. An fMRI study of imagined self-rotation , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[39] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Human Body , 2001 .
[40] G. Humphreys,et al. Calling a squirrel a squirrel but a canoe a wigwam: a category-specific deficit for artefactual objects and body parts , 1992 .
[41] Simone Rossi,et al. TMS in cognitive plasticity and the potential for rehabilitation , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[42] J. Talairach,et al. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .
[43] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] M. D’Esposito,et al. Category-specific modulation of inferior temporal activity during working memory encoding and maintenance. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.