Optimistic bias and food

Food consumption patterns are influenced by a number of factors, including social and cultural factors. It is difficult to effect dietary change, and one possible barrier to dietary change is optimistic bias. Research indicates that individuals tend to believe that they are less likely to experience negative events, and more likely to experience positive events than their peers; this phenomenon is known as optimistic bias. It has been argued that optimistic bias may have a negative impact both on self-protective behaviour and on efforts to promote risk-reducing behaviours. The present article reviews the literature investigating optimistic bias specifically in the food domain. The review indicated that many food and nutrition issues are associated with optimistic bias. This has important implications for health-promotion activities in the food domain. The paper also describes key aspects of the methodology used to investigate optimistic bias and details the conditions under which optimistic bias has been demonstrated. The importance of identifying the causes of optimistic bias is discussed, and empirical attempts designed to reduce optimistic bias by countering the causes are reviewed. Finally, directions for future research are suggested.

[1]  M. Kemeny,et al.  Realistic acceptance as a predictor of decreased survival time in gay men with AIDS. , 1994, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[2]  R Shepherd,et al.  Methodological Approaches to Assessing Risk Perceptions Associated with Food‐Related Hazards , 1998, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[3]  Optimism in Children's Judgments of Health and Environmental Risks , 1994 .

[4]  Is optimistic bias influenced by control or delay? , 2001, Health education research.

[5]  Julian Peto,et al.  Cancer epidemiology in the last century and the next decade , 2001, Nature.

[6]  Valerie A. Clarke,et al.  Unrealistic Optimism and the Health Belief Model , 2000, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[7]  Skin cancer: Assessing perceived risk and behavioural attitudes , 1993 .

[8]  R. O'Neil,et al.  Optimism in children's judgments of health and environmental risks. , 1994, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[9]  Lisa G. Aspinwall,et al.  Distinguishing Optimism from Denial: Optimistic Beliefs Predict Attention to Health Threats , 1996 .

[10]  Susan Miles,et al.  Investigating specific concerns about different food hazards , 2001 .

[11]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  J. Kulik,et al.  Health status, perceptions of risk, and prevention interest for health and nonhealth problems. , 1987, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[13]  J. Eiser,et al.  Out in the midday Sun: Risk behaviour and optimistic beliefs among residents and visitors on tenerife , 1999 .

[14]  V. Clarke,et al.  Optimistic Bias in Beliefs about Smoking , 1997 .

[15]  D M DeJoy,et al.  The optimism bias and traffic accident risk perception. , 1989, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[16]  W. Klein MAINTAINING SELF-SERVING SOCIAL COMPARISONS: ATTENUATING THE PERCEIVED SIGNIFICANCE OF RISK-INCREASING BEHAVIORS , 1996 .

[17]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility. , 1984, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[18]  M. Kemeny,et al.  Optimism is associated with mood, coping, and immune change in response to stress. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Bram P. Buunk,et al.  SOCIAL-COMPARISON OF HEALTH RISKS - LOCUS OF CONTROL, THE PERSON-POSITIVITY BIAS, AND UNREALISTIC OPTIMISM , 1993 .

[20]  Richard Shepherd,et al.  Perceived behavioural control, unrealistic optimism and dietary change: An exploratory study , 1995, Appetite.

[21]  R Shepherd,et al.  Public perceptions of the potential hazards associated with food production and food consumption: an empirical study. , 1994, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[22]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Optimism, coping, psychological distress, and high-risk sexual behavior among men at risk for acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Unrealistic optimism and genetic risk , 1996 .

[24]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Mindset, optimistic bias about personal risk and health-protective behaviour , 1999 .

[25]  Vera Hoorens,et al.  Self‐Favoring Biases, Self‐Presentation, and the Self‐Other Asymmetry in Social Comparison , 1995 .

[26]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Perceptions of smoking risk as a function of smoking status , 1992, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[27]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Reducing unrealistic optimism about illness susceptibility. , 1983 .

[28]  G. Bishop,et al.  Perceived vulnerability to illness threats: The role of disease type, risk factor perception and attributions , 1995 .

[29]  M. L. Klotz,et al.  Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .

[30]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  K. Fontaine,et al.  Optimistic Bias in Cancer Risk Perception: A Cross-National Study , 1995, Psychological reports.

[32]  R Shepherd,et al.  The effects of providing personalized dietary feedback. A semi-computerized approach. , 1999, Patient education and counseling.

[33]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Situated Optimism: Specific Outcome Expectancies and Self-Regulation , 1998 .

[34]  B. Meyerowitz,et al.  Positive expectations predict health after heart transplantation. , 1995, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[35]  Bram P. Buunk,et al.  Health, Coping, and Well-being : Perspectives From Social Comparison Theory , 1997 .

[36]  Mark Snyder,et al.  Unrealistic Optimism: Self-Enhancement or Person Positivity? , 1995 .

[37]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems , 1982, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[38]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Egocentrism as a Source of Unrealistic Optimism , 1982 .

[39]  W. Klein,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Resistance of Personal Risk Perceptions to Debiasing Interventions , 2002 .

[40]  R Shepherd,et al.  Social determinants of food choice , 1999, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[41]  J. Burger,et al.  The Illusion of Unique Invulnerability and the Use of Effective Contraception , 1988, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[42]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Absolute and Relative Biases in Estimations of Personal Risk , 1996 .

[43]  P. Harris,et al.  Give ‘Em Enough Rope: Perception of Health and Safety Risks in Bungee Jumpers , 1996 .

[44]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Positive illusions and well-being revisited: separating fact from fiction. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Maintaining Self-Serving Social Comparisons: Biased Reconstruction of One's Past Behaviors , 1993 .

[46]  N. Weinstein Optimistic biases about personal risks. , 1989, Science.

[47]  M. Helweg-Larsen (The Lack of) Optimistic Biases in Response to the 1994 Northridge Earthquake: The Role of Personal Experience , 1999 .

[48]  J L Collins,et al.  Physical activity, food choice, and weight management goals and practices among US college students. , 2000, American journal of preventive medicine.

[49]  Health status, perceptions of risk, and prevention interest for health and nonhealth problems. , 1987 .

[50]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[51]  A. Åstrøm,et al.  Perceptions of susceptibility to oral health hazards: a study of women in different cultures. , 1998, Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology.

[52]  P. Sparks,et al.  Unrealistic optimism about diet-related risks: implications for interventions , 1995, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[53]  N. Weinstein The precaution adoption process. , 1988, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[54]  Richard Joiner,et al.  The typical student as an in‐group member: eliminating optimistic bias by reducing social distance , 2000 .

[55]  J. Eiser,et al.  Participation in Cervical Screening as a Function of Perceived Risk, Barriers and need for Cognitive Closure , 2002, Journal of health psychology.

[56]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1. , 1975, The Journal of psychology.

[57]  J. van der Pligt,et al.  Risk perception and behavior: pessimism, realism, and optimism about AIDS-related health behavior , 1992 .

[58]  Paul Sparks,et al.  Expectations of reducing fat intake: The role of perceived need within the theory of planned behaviour , 1998 .

[59]  Peter R. Harris,et al.  Sufficient grounds for optimism? The relationship between perceived controllability and optimistic bias , 1996 .

[60]  N. Miller Some main themes and highlights of the conference. , 1983 .

[61]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample , 1987, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[62]  C. C. Chandler,et al.  It can't happen to me… or can it? Conditional base rates affect subjective probability judgments , 1999 .

[63]  Wilma Otten,et al.  Context effects in the measurement of optimism in probability judgment , 1996 .

[64]  F. McKenna,et al.  Illusory self-assessments--can they be reduced? , 1997, British journal of psychology.

[65]  N. Weinstein Testing four competing theories of health-protective behavior. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[66]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Testing four competing theories of health-protective behavior. , 1993 .

[67]  Illusions of well-being and the repressive coping style. , 1996, The British journal of social psychology.

[68]  P. Harris,et al.  The illusion of control and optimism about health: on being less at risk but no more in control than others. , 1994, The British journal of social psychology.

[69]  L. Perloff,et al.  Self–other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. , 1986 .

[70]  Kenneth M. Prkachin,et al.  Optimism and Unrealistic Optimism have an Interacting Impact on Health-Promoting Behavior and Knowledge Changes , 1997 .

[71]  A. Tversky Features of Similarity , 1977 .

[72]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Increasing the Salience of Risky Sexual Behavior: Promoting Interest in HIV‐Antibody Testing Among Heterosexually Active Young Adults1 , 1999 .

[73]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Do Moderators of the Optimistic Bias Affect Personal or Target Risk Estimates? A Review of the Literature , 2001 .

[74]  C. Peterson,et al.  Optimistic explanatory style and the perception of health problems. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[75]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later , 1984, Health education quarterly.

[76]  M. Becker,et al.  Bringing the models together: An empirical approach to combining variables used to explain health actions , 1980, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[77]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Changes in Perceived Vulnerability Following Natural Disaster , 2000 .

[78]  Lynn J. Frewer,et al.  THE INTERRELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PERCEIVED KNOWLEDGE, CONTROL AND RISK ASSOCIATED WITH A RANGE OF FOOD‐RELATED HAZARDS TARGETED AT THE INDIVIDUAL, OTHER PEOPLE AND SOCIETY , 1994 .

[79]  W. Klein,et al.  Social comparison and unrealistic optimism about personal risk. , 1997 .