Separate representations of dynamics in rhythmic and discrete movements: evidence from motor learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Stephen H Scott,et al. Limited transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual skills within the same limb , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[2] H. Haken,et al. A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[3] S. Scott,et al. Multi-compartment model can explain partial transfer of learning within the same limb between unimanual and bimanual reaching , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] Masaya Hirashima,et al. Asymmetric Transfer of Visuomotor Learning between Discrete and Rhythmic Movements , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] Stefan Schaal,et al. Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements , 2000 .
[6] William J. Dean,et al. Interaction of discrete and rhythmic movements over a wide range of periods , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] S. Schaal,et al. Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[8] D. Wolpert,et al. Context-dependent partitioning of motor learning in bimanual movements. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[9] Keng Peng Tee,et al. Concurrent adaptation of force and impedance in the redundant muscle system , 2010, Biological Cybernetics.
[10] J. Duysens,et al. The breakdown of Fitts’ law in rapid, reciprocal aiming movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[11] E. Saltzman,et al. Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. , 1987 .
[12] C. Shea,et al. Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: switching between cyclical and discrete units of action , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Does the motor control system use multiple models and context switching to cope with a variable environment? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] J. Kelso. Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.
[15] Werner Wolf,et al. The discontinuous nature of motor execution , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.
[16] Daniel B. Willingham,et al. Patterns of interference in sequence learning and prism adaptation inconsistent with the consolidation hypothesis. , 2002, Learning & memory.
[17] Jeremy D Wong,et al. Visual cues signaling object grasp reduce interference in motor learning. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[18] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] Dagmar Sternad,et al. Optimal control of a hybrid rhythmic-discrete task: the bouncing ball revisited. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] J. Kalaska,et al. Context-dependent anticipation of different task dynamics: rapid recall of appropriate motor skills using visual cues. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. A modular planar robotic manipulandum with end-point torque control , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[22] Dagmar Sternad,et al. Debates in dynamics: A dynamical systems perspective on action and perception , 2000 .
[23] Stefan Schaal,et al. Dynamics systems vs. optimal control--a unifying view. , 2007, Progress in brain research.
[24] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[25] Werner Wolf,et al. The discontinuous nature of motor execution II. Merging discrete and rhythmic movements in a single-joint system – the phase entrainment effect , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.
[26] R. Miall,et al. Adaptation to rotated visual feedback: a re-examination of motor interference , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[27] R.,et al. Interaction of Discrete and Continuous parts of aBatch Process Control , 1995 .
[28] Dagmar Sternad,et al. Rhythmic and discrete elements in multi-joint coordination , 2003, Brain Research.
[29] Andrew W. Fitzgibbon,et al. Direct Least Square Fitting of Ellipses , 1999, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[30] Ian S Howard,et al. Composition and Decomposition in Bimanual Dynamic Learning , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] D M Wolpert,et al. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.
[32] P. Beek,et al. Discrete and cyclical movements: unified dynamics or separate control? , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[33] R. Miall,et al. Moving to a different beat , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[34] Otmar Bock,et al. Conditions for interference versus facilitation during sequential sensorimotor adaptation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] J. Krakauer,et al. Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] N. Hogan,et al. On rhythmic and discrete movements: reflections, definitions and implications for motor control , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] E. Bizzi,et al. Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.
[38] N. Schweighofer,et al. Dual Adaptation Supports a Parallel Architecture of Motor Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[39] F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] T. Brashers-Krug,et al. Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[41] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. A Computational Model for Rhythmic and Discrete Movements in Uni- and Bimanual Coordination , 2009, Neural Computation.
[42] John W. Krakauer,et al. Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] G. Schöner,et al. A dynamic theory of coordination of discrete movement , 1990, Biological Cybernetics.
[44] J Randall Flanagan,et al. Visuomotor rotations of varying size and direction compete for a single internal model in motor working memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.