About the influence of post-saccadic mechanisms for visual stability on peri-saccadic compression of object location.

Peri-saccadic perception experiments have revealed a multitude of mislocalization phenomena. For instance, a briefly flashed stimulus is perceived closer to the saccade target, whereas a displacement of the saccade target goes usually unnoticeable. This latter saccadic suppression of displacement has been explained by a built-in characteristic of the perceptual system: the assumption that during a saccade, the environment remains stable. We explored whether the mislocalization of a briefly flashed stimulus toward the saccade target also grounds in the built-in assumption of a stable environment. If the mislocalization of a peri-saccadically flashed stimulus originates from a post-saccadic alignment process, an additional location marker at the position of the upcoming flash should counteract compression. Alternatively, compression might be the result of peri-saccadic attentional phenomena. In this case, mislocalization should occur even if the position of the flashed stimulus is marked. When subjects were asked about their perceived location, they mislocalized the stimulus toward the saccade target, even though they were fully aware of the correct stimulus location. Thus, our results suggest that the uncertainty about the location of a flashed stimulus is not inherently relevant for compression.

[1]  L. Matin,et al.  Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[2]  N. Bischof,et al.  [Investigations and considerations of directional perception during voluntary saccadic eye movements]. , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[3]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[4]  F. Campbell,et al.  Saccadic omission: Why we do not see a grey-out during a saccadic eye movement , 1978, Vision Research.

[5]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective depression of motion sensitivity during saccades. , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  H. Honda Saccade-contingent displacement of the apparent position of visual stimuli flashed on a dimly illuminated structured background , 1993, Vision Research.

[8]  J. Schlag,et al.  Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement , 1996, Vision Research.

[10]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[11]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Perceived geometrical relationships affected by eye-movement signals , 1997, Nature.

[12]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy , 1998, Vision Research.

[14]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[15]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Transsaccadic memory of position and form. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[17]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Through the eye, slowly; Delays and localization errors in the visual system , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal transsaccadic integration explains distorted spatial perception , 2003, Nature.

[19]  F. Hamker The reentry hypothesis: linking eye movements to visual perception. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[20]  M. Lappe,et al.  Contrast dependency of saccadic compression and suppression , 2004, Vision Research.

[21]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Different effects of eyelid blinks and target blanking on saccadic suppression of displacement , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Anna Ma-Wyatt,et al.  Seeing and ballistic pointing at perisaccadic targets. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[23]  F. Hamker The reentry hypothesis: the putative interaction of the frontal eye field, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and areas V4, IT for attention and eye movement. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal inference explains dimension-specific contractions of spatial perception , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Mislocalization of Perceived Saccade Target Position Induced by Perisaccadic Visual Stimulation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Eli Brenner,et al.  The role of uncertainty in the systematic spatial mislocalization of moving objects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  David C Burr,et al.  Fusion of Visual and Auditory Stimuli during Saccades: A Bayesian Explanation for Perisaccadic Distortions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Efference copy and its limitations , 2007, Comput. Biol. Medicine.

[29]  Markus Lappe,et al.  The Peri-Saccadic Perception of Objects and Space , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[30]  F. Hamker,et al.  Influence of adaptation state and stimulus luminance on peri-saccadic localization. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[31]  K. J.,et al.  Retinal versus extraretinal influences in flash localization during saccadic eye movements in the presence of a visible background , 2022 .