Using social information to guide action: Infants' locomotion over slippery slopes

In uncertain situations such as descending challenging slopes, social signals from caregivers can provide infants with important information for guiding action. Previous work showed that 18-month-old walking infants use social information selectively, only when risk of falling is uncertain. Experiment 1 was designed to alter infants' region of uncertainty for walking down slopes. Slippery Teflon-soled shoes drastically impaired 18-month-olds' ability to walk down slopes compared with walking barefoot or in standard crepe-soled shoes, shifting the region of uncertainty to a shallower range of slopes. In Experiment 2, infants wore Teflon-soled shoes while walking down slopes as their mothers encouraged and discouraged them from walking. Infants relied on social information on shallow slopes, even at 0°, where the probability of walking successfully was uncertain in the Teflon-soled shoes. Findings indicate that infants' use of social information is dynamically attuned to situational factors and the state of their current abilities.

[1]  Karen E Adolph,et al.  Gauging possibilities for action based on friction underfoot. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  J. Richards,et al.  Affective, behavioral, and avoidance responses on the visual cliff: effects of crawling onset age, crawling experience, and testing age. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[3]  E. Gibson,et al.  A comparative and analytical study of visual depth perception. , 1961 .

[4]  Megan R. Gunnar,et al.  The effects of maternal positive, neutral and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys , 1987 .

[5]  W. Damon,et al.  Social, emotional, and personality development , 1998 .

[6]  T. Striano,et al.  Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds' social referencing behavior. , 2004, Developmental science.

[7]  J. Richards,et al.  On the nature of the visual-cliff-avoidance response in human infants. , 1980, Child development.

[8]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Infants' perception of affordances of slopes under high- and low-friction conditions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  R. D. Walk,et al.  The development of depth perception in animals and human infants. , 1966, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[10]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  Infants' responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. , 1996, Child development.

[11]  S. Feinman,et al.  A Critical Review of Social Referencing in Infancy , 1992 .

[12]  Gunilla Stenberg,et al.  Infant Looking Behavior in Ambiguous Situations: Social Referencing or Attachment Behavior?. , 2007 .

[13]  J. Richards,et al.  Crawling-onset age predicts visual cliff avoidance in infants. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Gunilla Stenberg,et al.  Social referencing and mood modification in 1-year-olds , 1996 .

[15]  Anthony M. Avolio,et al.  Walking infants adapt locomotion to changing body dimensions. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Gunilla Stenberg,et al.  Selectivity in Infant Social Referencing. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[17]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Travel Broadens the Mind. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[18]  B. Homer,et al.  The Development of Social Cognition and Communication , 2005 .

[19]  J. Campos,et al.  Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. , 1985 .

[20]  Karen E Adolph,et al.  When infants take mothers' advice: 18-month-olds integrate perceptual and social information to guide motor action. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[21]  L. Hirshberg,et al.  When infants look to their parents: I. Infants' social referencing of mothers compared to fathers. , 1990, Child development.

[22]  Karen E. Adolph,et al.  How Mothers Encourage and Discourage Infants' Motor Actions , 2008 .

[23]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Avoidance of Heights on the Visual Cliff in Newly Walking Infants. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[24]  L. Camras,et al.  Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding , 2007 .

[25]  M. McGraw,et al.  Growth: A Study of Johnny and Jimmy , 1982 .

[26]  K. Adolph Specificity of Learning: Why Infants Fall Over a Veritable Cliff , 2000, Psychological science.

[27]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  The Ontogeny of Social Information Gathering , 1996 .

[28]  Tom Johnston,et al.  MacSHAPA and the enterprise of exploratory sequential data analysis (ESDA) , 1994, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[29]  Karen E Adolph,et al.  Locomotor experience and use of social information are posture specific. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[30]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[31]  D. Signorini,et al.  Neural networks , 1995, The Lancet.

[32]  K. Adolph,et al.  Learning in the development of infant locomotion. , 1997, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[33]  Harry F. Harlow,et al.  The human model : primate perspectives , 1979 .

[34]  B I Bertenthal,et al.  A reexamination of fear and its determinants on the visual cliff. , 1984, Psychophysiology.