Activation of Different Anterior Cingulate Foci in Association with Hypothesis Testing and Response Selection

Much everyday behavior is implicitly guided by hypotheses about the world which are monitored and updated in the light of changing circumstances. The process of translating these hypotheses into behavior typically involves implementing choices, often based on incompletely specified information. The present study aimed at modeling these processes to determine the neural substrates of hypothesis testing and, in particular, how these are modulated by the requirement to make choices. We used positron emission tomography to study six right-handed volunteers performing an insoluble hypothesis testing task in which subjects attempted to identify a rule determining which of two black and white checkerboard stimuli was correct. This task was compared with a control task matched for perceptuomotor requirements, but involving no hypothesis testing. Both tasks were performed with or without a requirement to make a choice. Structures activated in association with hypothesis testing included the cerebellum, left anterior cingulate, right precuneus, right thalamus, and left inferior frontal gyrus. The requirement to choose a response was associated with activation of the left anterior cingulate and right lateral orbitofrontal cortex. A significant modulation of activation associated with hypothesis testing was observed in the anterior cingulate region that was also activated by making a choice. These findings are discussed in terms of the neural substrates of complex "executive" tasks. We argue that the precise cognitive parameters of such tasks, and specifically the requirement to implement decisions in actual behavior, are critical in determining the associated neural response.

[1]  B. Kushner Descartes' error. , 1998, Journal of AAPOS : the official publication of the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus.

[2]  Otto Muzik,et al.  Consistency analysis for digitized 3D Talairach's coplanar stereotactic atlas of the human brain , 1997, Photonics West - Biomedical Optics.

[3]  D. Pandya,et al.  Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  M. Petrides,et al.  Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[5]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of human concept formation skills , 1996, NeuroImage.

[7]  J. Bower,et al.  Cerebellum Implicated in Sensory Acquisition and Discrimination Rather Than Motor Control , 1996, Science.

[8]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[11]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Isolating the Mnemonic Component in Spatial Delayed Response: A Controlled PET15O-Labeled Water Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Study in Normal Humans , 1996, NeuroImage.

[12]  J. Schmahmann From movement to thought: Anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognitive processing , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[13]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The Mind's Eye—Precuneus Activation in Memory-Related Imagery , 1995, NeuroImage.

[14]  Richard Coppola,et al.  Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A positron emission tomography study , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human ventrolateral frontal cortex during mnemonic retrieval of verbal information. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[17]  P M Grasby,et al.  Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. An in vivo study in humans. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  B. Gulyás,et al.  Visual imagery and visual representation , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  S. Haber,et al.  Primate cingulostriatal projection: Limbic striatal versus sensorimotor striatal input , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[20]  P. Strick,et al.  Activation of a cerebellar output nucleus during cognitive processing. , 1994, Science.

[21]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[23]  W. Bank The Human Brain. Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy and MRI , 1993 .

[24]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  N C Andreasen,et al.  Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Assessment with xenon 133 single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of London. , 1992, Archives of general psychiatry.

[29]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Cingulate input to the primary and supplementary motor cortices in the rhesus monkey: Evidence for somatotopy in areas 24c and 23c , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  M. Hallett,et al.  Cognitive planning deficit in patients with cerebellar atrophy , 1992, Neurology.

[31]  S. Petersen,et al.  Impaired non-motor learning and error detection associated with cerebellar damage. A single case study. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  C. Saper,et al.  Efferent projections of the infralimbic cortex of the rat , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  RP Dum,et al.  The origin of corticospinal projections from the premotor areas in the frontal lobe , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[37]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. , 1988, Science.

[39]  P. T. Fox,et al.  Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing , 1988, Nature.

[40]  D L Rosene,et al.  Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: I. Cytoarchitecture and thalamic afferents , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[41]  D. Pandya,et al.  Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: II. Cortical afferents , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[42]  E. Neafsey,et al.  Rat medial frontal cortex: a visceral motor region with a direct projection to the solitary nucleus , 1983, Brain Research.

[43]  John Cohen,et al.  Psychological Probability, Or, The Art Of Doubt , 1972 .

[44]  G. Bower,et al.  PRESOLUTION REVERSAL AND DIMENSIONAL SHIFTS IN CONCEPT IDENTIFICATION. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.