Neural signatures of model-free learning when avoiding harm to self and other
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Wouter Kool,et al. Reasoning supports utilitarian resolutions to moral dilemmas across diverse measures. , 2020, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[2] B. Seymour. Pain: A Precision Signal for Reinforcement Learning and Control , 2019, Neuron.
[3] Jo Cutler,et al. A comparative fMRI meta-analysis of altruistic and strategic decisions to give , 2019, NeuroImage.
[4] Marco K. Wittmann,et al. Neural mechanisms for learning self and other ownership , 2018, Nature Communications.
[5] M. Crockett,et al. The lateral prefrontal cortex and moral goal pursuit. , 2018, Current opinion in psychology.
[6] Marco K. Wittmann,et al. Ventral anterior cingulate cortex and social decision-making , 2018, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[7] Sang Wan Lee,et al. Model-based and model-free pain avoidance learning , 2018, Brain and neuroscience advances.
[8] Matthew F S Rushworth,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Social Cognition in Primates. , 2018, Annual review of neuroscience.
[9] M. Crockett,et al. Beyond Sacrificial Harm: A Two-Dimensional Model of Utilitarian Psychology , 2017, Psychological review.
[10] R. Dolan,et al. Neural and computational processes underlying dynamic changes in self-esteem , 2017, eLife.
[11] P. Railton. Moral Learning: Conceptual foundations and normative relevance , 2017, Cognition.
[12] P. Railton,et al. Moral learning: Psychological and philosophical perspectives , 2017, Cognition.
[13] Joshua D. Greene. The rat-a-gorical imperative: Moral intuition and the limits of affective learning , 2017, Cognition.
[14] M. Husain,et al. Prosocial apathy for helping others when effort is required , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[15] E. Koechlin,et al. The Importance of Falsification in Computational Cognitive Modeling , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[16] Jenifer Z. Siegel,et al. Moral transgressions corrupt neural representations of value , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.
[17] J. Decety,et al. Interpersonal harm aversion as a necessary foundation for morality: A developmental neuroscience perspective , 2017, Development and Psychopathology.
[18] Fiery Cushman,et al. Morality constrains the default representation of what is possible , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[19] M. Husain,et al. Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying subjective valuation of effort costs , 2017, PLoS biology.
[20] Wolfgang M. Pauli,et al. Learning, Reward, and Decision Making , 2017, Annual review of psychology.
[21] Vincent D Costa,et al. Amygdala and Ventral Striatum Make Distinct Contributions to Reinforcement Learning , 2016, Neuron.
[22] Essi Viding,et al. Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[23] Wouter Kool,et al. When Does Model-Based Control Pay Off? , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[24] Karl J. Friston,et al. Neural Signatures of Value Comparison in Human Cingulate Cortex during Decisions Requiring an Effort-Reward Trade-off , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[25] Hans Knutsson,et al. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] Alisabeth Ayars. Can model-free reinforcement learning explain deontological moral judgments? , 2016, Cognition.
[27] P. Dayan,et al. Striatal structure and function predict individual biases in learning to avoid pain , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[28] N. Daw,et al. Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control , 2016, eLife.
[29] J. V. Bavel,et al. The Neuroscience of Moral Cognition: From Dual Processes to Dynamic Systems , 2015 .
[30] Jenifer Z. Siegel,et al. Dissociable Effects of Serotonin and Dopamine on the Valuation of Harm in Moral Decision Making , 2015, Current Biology.
[31] Simon B. Eickhoff,et al. Functional organization of human subgenual cortical areas: Relationship between architectonical segregation and connectional heterogeneity , 2015, NeuroImage.
[32] M. Crockett,et al. Goal-directed, habitual and Pavlovian prosocial behavior , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[33] Dean Mobbs,et al. Empathic concern drives costly altruism , 2015, NeuroImage.
[34] Jenifer Z. Siegel,et al. Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[35] F. Cushman,et al. Bad actions or bad outcomes? Differentiating affective contributions to the moral condemnation of harm. , 2014, Emotion.
[36] P. Dayan,et al. Correction: Disentangling the Roles of Approach, Activation and Valence in Instrumental and Pavlovian Responding , 2014, PLoS Computational Biology.
[37] Alice Y. Chiang,et al. Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[38] Jason P. Mitchell,et al. Intuitive Prosociality , 2013 .
[39] Thomas H. B. FitzGerald,et al. Disruption of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Decreases Model-Based in Favor of Model-free Control in Humans , 2013, Neuron.
[40] E. Fehr,et al. Changing Social Norm Compliance with Noninvasive Brain Stimulation , 2013, Science.
[41] P. Dayan,et al. Goals and Habits in the Brain , 2013, Neuron.
[42] R. Blair. The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[43] F. Cushman. Action, Outcome, and Value , 2013, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[44] M. Crockett. Models of morality , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[45] Adam G. Thomas,et al. The Organization of Dorsal Frontal Cortex in Humans and Macaques , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[46] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Neural Computations Underlying Arbitration between Model-Based and Model-free Learning , 2013, Neuron.
[47] W. Schultz. Updating dopamine reward signals , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[48] Dean Mobbs,et al. Deconstructing the brain’s moral network: dissociable functionality between the temporoparietal junction and ventro-medial prefrontal cortex , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[49] R. Dolan,et al. Dopamine Enhances Model-Based over Model-Free Choice Behavior , 2012, Neuron.
[50] M. Rushworth,et al. Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right "temporoparietal junction area": evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[51] Alberto Priori,et al. Functional and clinical neuroanatomy of morality. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[52] Russell Thompson,et al. Differential neural circuitry and self-interest in real vs hypothetical moral decisions , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[53] R. Marois,et al. The roots of modern justice: cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[54] Kurt Gray,et al. Mind Perception Is the Essence of Morality , 2012, Psychological inquiry.
[55] Peter Dayan,et al. Bonsai Trees in Your Head: How the Pavlovian System Sculpts Goal-Directed Choices by Pruning Decision Trees , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[56] P. Dayan,et al. Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[57] Fiery Cushman,et al. Simulating murder: the aversion to harmful action. , 2012, Emotion.
[58] Luke J. Chang,et al. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion , 2011, Neuron.
[59] Raymond J. Dolan,et al. Disentangling the Roles of Approach, Activation and Valence in Instrumental and Pavlovian Responding , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[60] P. Dayan,et al. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.
[61] S. Levinson,et al. WEIRD languages have misled us, too , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[62] P. Dayan,et al. States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning , 2010, Neuron.
[63] J. Henrich,et al. The weirdest people in the world? , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[64] R. Saxe,et al. Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[65] I. Kant. The Metaphysic of Ethics , 2009 .
[66] Joseph M. Paxton,et al. Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[67] B. Balleine,et al. A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[68] Jorge Moll,et al. Social attachment and aversion in human moral cognition , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[69] D. Ariely,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Contagion and Differentiation in Unethical Behavior The Effect of One Bad Apple on the Barrel , 2022 .
[70] P. Dayan,et al. Decision theory, reinforcement learning, and the brain , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[71] Noah J. Goldstein,et al. Normative Social Influence is Underdetected , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[72] Russell A. Poldrack,et al. Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study , 2008, NeuroImage.
[73] U. Fischbacher,et al. The Neural Signature of Social Norm Compliance , 2007, Neuron.
[74] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. Diminishing Reciprocal Fairness by Disrupting the Right Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Science.
[75] J. O'Doherty,et al. Is Avoiding an Aversive Outcome Rewarding? Neural Substrates of Avoidance Learning in the Human Brain , 2006, PLoS biology.
[76] P. Dayan,et al. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[77] J. Grafman,et al. The neural basis of human moral cognition , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[78] J. O'Doherty,et al. Human Neural Learning Depends on Reward Prediction Errors in the Blocking Paradigm , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[79] Andrew D. Engell,et al. The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.
[80] D. Price. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. , 2000, Science.
[81] Richard E. Tremblay,et al. The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: What have we learned in the past century? , 2000 .
[82] Anthony K. P. Jones,et al. The cortical representation of pain , 1999, PAIN.
[83] H Nishijo,et al. Emotional and Behavioral Correlates of Mediodorsal Thalamic Neurons during Associative Learning in Rats , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[84] D. Gaffan,et al. Amygdalar interaction with the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in stimulus-reward associative learning in the monkey , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[85] A. Ohman,et al. One-trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[86] John C. Harsanyi,et al. Cardinal Welfare, Individualistic Ethics, and Interpersonal Comparisons of Utility , 1955, Journal of Political Economy.
[87] J. Bentham. An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation , 1945, Princeton Readings in Political Thought.
[88] Dan Ariely,et al. The Effect of One Bad Apple on the Barrel , 2009 .
[89] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[90] Bernard Gert,et al. Common Morality: Deciding What to Do , 2004 .
[91] T. Nagel. Mortal Questions: What is it like to be a bat? , 2012 .
[92] M. Pritchard. ON COMMON MORALITY , 2001 .
[93] F. Lenz,et al. Stimulation in the human somatosensory thalamus can reproduce both the affective and sensory dimensions of previously experienced pain , 1995, Nature Network Boston.
[94] Model Comparison and Occam ’ s Razor , 2022 .