Audio-tactile cues from an object’s fall change estimates of one’s body height

When we drop an object from our hands, we use internal models of both our body height and object-motion to predict when it will hit the floor. What happens if the sensory feedback finally received from the impact conflicts with this prediction? The present study shows that such conflict results in changes in the internal estimates of our body height: When the object people dropped takes longer than expected to hit the floor, they report feeling taller and behave as if their legs were longer. This provides the first evidence of cross-modal recalibration of body-height representations as a function of changes in the distant environment. Crucially, the recalibration results from a mismatch between the predicted and actual outcome of an action, the ball’s release and impact, which are causally-related but separated in space and time. These results suggest that implicit models of object-motion can interact with implicit and explicit models of one’s body height.

[1]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Adaptation to suppression of visual information during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[3]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Passingham,et al.  That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb , 2004, Science.

[5]  S. Nishida,et al.  Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Dora E. Angelaki,et al.  Neurons compute internal models of the physical laws of motion , 2004, Nature.

[7]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  A. Maravita,et al.  Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Representation of Visual Gravitational Motion in the Human Vestibular Cortex , 2005, Science.

[10]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Experimenting with the acting self , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[11]  B. Röder,et al.  Spatial remapping of touch: confusion of perceived stimulus order across hand and foot. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  T. Metzinger,et al.  Video Ergo Sum: Manipulating Bodily Self-Consciousness , 2007, Science.

[13]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models and prediction of visual gravitational motion , 2008, Vision Research.

[14]  Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al.  What is embodiment? A psychometric approach , 2008, Cognition.

[15]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Interactions of different body parts in peripersonal space: how vision of the foot influences tactile perception at the hand , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Vincenzo Maffei,et al.  Vestibular nuclei and cerebellum put visual gravitational motion in context. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Asymmetrical Body Perception , 2009, Psychological science.

[18]  P. Haggard,et al.  Rubber Hand Illusions and Size–Weight Illusions: Self-Representation Modulates Representation of External Objects , 2009, Perception.

[19]  P. Haggard,et al.  Distorting the visual size of the hand affects hand pre-shaping during grasping , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  P. Haggard,et al.  Touch and the body , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  H. Henrik Ehrsson,et al.  Being Barbie: The Size of One’s Own Body Determines the Perceived Size of the World , 2011, PloS one.

[22]  Manos Tsakiris,et al.  Just a heartbeat away from one's body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  What Is It Like to Have a Body? , 2012 .

[24]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Extending Body Space in Immersive Virtual Reality: A Very Long Arm Illusion , 2012, PloS one.

[25]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  Action sounds recalibrate perceived tactile distance , 2012, Current Biology.

[26]  Alisa Mandrigin,et al.  Bodily ownership and self-location: Components of bodily self-consciousness , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[27]  Marion Koopmans,et al.  Profiling of Humoral Response to Influenza A(H1N1)pdm09 Infection and Vaccination Measured by a Protein Microarray in Persons with and without History of Seasonal Vaccination , 2013, PloS one.

[28]  Albert Postma,et al.  Too Fat to Fit through the Door: First Evidence for Disturbed Body-Scaled Action in Anorexia Nervosa during Locomotion , 2013, PloS one.

[29]  D. Alais,et al.  Rapid Recalibration to Audiovisual Asynchrony , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  O. Blanke,et al.  The effect of limb crossing and limb congruency on multisensory integration in peripersonal space for the upper and lower extremities , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[31]  M. Slater,et al.  Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  Action sounds update the mental representation of arm dimension: contributions of kinaesthesia and agency , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[33]  J. McGrane Stevens’ forgotten crossroads: the divergent measurement traditions in the physical and psychological sciences from the mid-twentieth century , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[34]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Those are Your Legs: The Effect of Visuo-Spatial Viewpoint on Visuo-Tactile Integration and Body Ownership , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[35]  M. Longo Implicit and Explicit Body Representations , 2015 .

[36]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Direction-dependent arm kinematics reveal optimal integration of gravity cues , 2016, eLife.

[37]  Jean-Paul Noel,et al.  Relative contributions of visual and auditory spatial representations to tactile localization , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Virtual reality improves embodiment and neuropathic pain caused by spinal cord injury , 2017, Neurology.

[39]  N. Bianchi-Berthouze,et al.  Contingent sounds change the mental representation of one’s finger length , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[40]  M. Wallace,et al.  Event Related Potentials Index Rapid Recalibration to Audiovisual Temporal Asynchrony , 2017, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[41]  H. C. Dijkerman,et al.  Peripersonal space boundaries around the lower limbs , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Thomas Metzinger,et al.  Unconscious integration of multisensory bodily inputs in the peripersonal space shapes bodily self-consciousness , 2016, Cognition.