Grouping local orientation and direction signals to extract spatial contours: Empirical tests of “association field” models of contour integration

Over the last decade or so a great deal of psychophysical research has attempted to delineate the principles by which local orientations and motions are combined across space to facilitate the detection of simple spatial contours. This has led to the development of "association field" models of contour detection which suggest that the strength of linking between neighbouring elements in an image, is determined by the degree to which they aligned along smooth (first-order) curves. To test this assumption we used a path detection paradigm to compare the ability of observers to identify the presence of contours defined by either spatial orientation, motion direction or by specific combinations of both types of visual attribute. The relative alignment of the local orientations and/or directions with respect to the axis of the depicted contour was systematically varied. For orientation-defined contours detection was best when the elements were aligned along (parallel with) the contour axis, approached chance levels for obliquely oriented elements and then improved for elements that were orthogonal to the contour axis (i.e., performance was a U-shaped function of degree of orientation misalignment). This pattern of results was found for both straight and curved contours and is not readily explicable in terms of current association field theories. For motion-defined contours, however, performance simply deteriorated as the relative directions of the constituent path elements were progressively misaligned with respect to the contour. Thus the rules by which local orientations are linked to define spatial contours are qualitatively different from those used for linking local directions and each may be mediated by distinct visual mechanisms. When both orientation and motion cues were simultaneously available, contour detection performance was generally enhanced, in a manner that is consistent with probability summation. We suggest that association field models of orientation linking may need to be extended in light of the present findings.

[1]  M. Wertheimer Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. II , 1923 .

[2]  W Singer,et al.  The Perceptual Grouping Criterion of Colinearity is Reflected by Anisotropies of Connections in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[4]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  L McKee,et al.  Snakes and ladders. , 1998, Nursing times.

[6]  C. Gilbert,et al.  On a common circle: natural scenes and Gestalt rules. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  D. Field,et al.  Integration of contours: new insights , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  M. Morgan,et al.  Facilitation from collinear flanks is cancelled by non-collinear flanks , 2000, Vision Research.

[9]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[10]  Norbert Krüger,et al.  Collinearity and Parallelism are Statistically Significant Second-Order Relations of Complex Cell Responses , 1998, Neural Processing Letters.

[11]  S. McKee,et al.  Detecting a trajectory embedded in random-direction motion noise , 1995, Vision Research.

[12]  J. Lund,et al.  Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[13]  Mauro Ursino,et al.  A model of contextual interactions and contour detection in primary visual cortex , 2004, Neural Networks.

[14]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Spatial summation and its interaction with the temporal integration mechanism in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[15]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[16]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Morphology and intracortical projections of functionally characterised neurones in the cat visual cortex , 1979, Nature.

[17]  T. S. Lee,et al.  Gestalten of Today: Early Processing of Visual Contours and Surfaces , 1996 .

[18]  Timothy Ledgeway,et al.  The detection of direction-defined and speed-defined spatial contours: one mechanism or two? , 2003, Vision Research.

[19]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Long-range horizontal connections and their role in cortical reorganization revealed by optical recording of cat primary visual cortex , 1995, Nature.

[20]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  A Neural Model of Contour Integration in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1998, Neural Computation.

[21]  Max Wertheimer,et al.  Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt , .

[22]  B. Julesz,et al.  Displacement limits, directional anisotropy and direction versus form discrimination in random-dot cinematograms , 1983, Vision Research.

[23]  S. Dakin,et al.  Grouping local directional signals into moving contours , 2003, Vision Research.

[24]  A. Grinvald,et al.  Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vivo targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortex , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  Accurate control of contrast on microcomputer displays , 1991, Vision Research.

[27]  Robert F Hess,et al.  Rules for combining the outputs of local motion detectors to define simple contours , 2002, Vision Research.

[28]  Jeffrey S. Perry,et al.  Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance , 2001, Vision Research.

[29]  L. Finkel,et al.  Extraction of perceptually salient contours by striate cortical networks , 1998, Vision Research.

[30]  Robert F Hess,et al.  Impoverished second-order input to global linking in human vision , 2000, Vision Research.

[31]  S. Maier,et al.  Widespread Periodic Intrinsic Connections in the Tree Shrew Visual Cortex , 2005 .

[32]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[33]  J. Elder,et al.  Ecological statistics of Gestalt laws for the perceptual organization of contours. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[34]  S P McKee,et al.  Stimulus configuration determines the detectability of motion signals in noise. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[35]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(98)00033-9 , 1998 .

[36]  S. Dakin,et al.  Snakes and ladders: the role of temporal modulation in visual contour integration , 2001, Vision Research.

[37]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[38]  Wu Li,et al.  Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.