Projection Bias in Medical Decision Making

however, my romantic misadventures had taught me that few people were as hooked on rationality as I was. More important, I had learned that you don’t get very far trying to make people more rational than they want to be. If we want to get people to change their behavior—and I am still persuaded that this can be a noble rather than a draconian ambition—we must develop the capacity to conceive what it “must be like” for them to think, feel, and behave the way they do. We must learn to see the world with their eyes and understand their values, motives, aims, categories, and concepts—an effort that may require us to at least temporarily suspend our most deep-seated assumptions about how they “should” behave. This too is no easy feat. But it is one that I am convinced the Society for Medical Decision-Making both must and can accomplish. From its inception 25 years ago, it has been committed to improving health and health care by showing people how to make rational use of medical knowledge. In a world with so many choices, and so much information to guide those choices, members of the Society have been leaders in developing and disseminating techniques for guiding health care decisions. They have set the standards in developing support tools—decision analyses and costeffective analyses—to help people maximize their health in the face of increasingly complex medical decisions. No single group of people is better equipped, therefore, to take the next step—namely, to incorporate into a model of decision-making the subtle but inevitable role that emotions play in the way that people arrive at decisions about their physical well-being. The 3 articles that follow, originally presented as talks at the Symposium on Emotions and Reason in Medical Decision Making, are ample evidence that the Society has already recognized and begun to address this challenge. Each, in their own way, illustrate how people’s decision-making processes do not match up to the standards that most of us champions of rationality might hope for. Like my ill-fated romantic experiences, the articles also illustrate that an exclusively rational approach, for all of its uses, may not be the one bestsuited to the matter at hand. In the 1st article, George Loewenstein discusses the ways in which people are influenced by the emotions of the moment, leading them to make radically different decisions depending on their mood. Loewenstein argues that, in effect, they become different people at different times. The 2nd article, by Norbert Schwarz, presents additional and compelling evidence of how people’s judgments and decisions are based not only on the information available to them, but also on their transitory moods and emotions. Schwarz’s research has momentous implications not only for showing how people think, but for demonstrating the challenges in collecting attitudinal information. The 3rd article, by Galen Bodenhausen, explores the role of stereotypes in decision-making. His article reminds us that decisions are not made in a cultural vacuum and that we need to consider not only the individual but also the social context of decision-making. Together, these 3 articles provide a firm scaffolding on which to construct a further and much-needed dialogue about the limits of rationality in understanding the decisions that people make about their health and their health care.

[1]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. , 2005, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[2]  David Dunning,et al.  The illusion of courage in social predictions: Underestimating the impact of fear of embarrassment on other people ! , 2005 .

[3]  D. Angus,et al.  Quality of Death: Assessing the Importance Placed on End-of-Life Treatment in the Intensive-Care Unit , 2004, Medical care.

[4]  A. Fagerlin,et al.  Enough. The failure of the living will. , 2004, The Hastings Center report.

[5]  George Loewenstein,et al.  Social Projection of Transient Drive States , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[6]  P. Berczeller ‘MY OWN MEDICINE: A Doctorʼs Life as a Patient’ , 2003 .

[7]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Regulation for Conservatives: Behavioral Economics and the Case for 'Asymmetric Paternalism' , 2003 .

[8]  Carol Ezzell,et al.  Why? The neuroscience of suicide , 2003 .

[9]  C. Ezzell New research addresses the wrenching question left when someone ends his or her own life. Why? The neuroscience of suicide. , 2003, Scientific American.

[10]  Jan Scott,et al.  Nonadherence with mood stabilizers: prevalence and predictors. , 2002, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[11]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  The Future Is Now: Temporal Correction in Affective Forecasting☆ , 2002 .

[12]  D. Elpern One hundred days: My unexpected journey from doctor to patient , 2001 .

[13]  Peter H. Ditto,et al.  Projection in surrogate decisions about life-sustaining medical treatments. , 2001, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[14]  R. Houts,et al.  Advance directives as acts of communication: a randomized controlled trial. , 2001, Archives of internal medicine.

[15]  Peter H. Ditto,et al.  Accuracy of primary care and hospital-based physicians' predictions of elderly outpatients' treatment preferences with and without advance directives. , 2001, Archives of internal medicine.

[16]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Projection Bias in Predicting Future Utility , 2000 .

[17]  Daniel Read,et al.  Enduring pain for money: decisions based on the perception and memory of pain , 1999 .

[18]  D. Read,et al.  Predicting Hunger: The Effects of Appetite and Delay on Choice. , 1998, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[19]  C. Gatsonis,et al.  Management of pain in elderly patients with cancer. SAGE Study Group. Systematic Assessment of Geriatric Drug Use via Epidemiology. , 1998, JAMA.

[20]  C. Cleeland Undertreatment of cancer pain in elderly patients. , 1998, JAMA.

[21]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  The role of decision analysis in informed consent: choosing between intuition and systematicity. , 1997, Social science & medicine.

[22]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  P. Ditto,et al.  Physicians' predictions of elderly outpatients' preferences for life-sustaining treatment. , 1993, The Journal of family practice.

[24]  W. Gregory,et al.  Attitudes to chemotherapy: comparing views of patients with cancer with those of doctors, nurses, and general public. , 1990, BMJ.

[25]  E. Rosenbaum A Taste of My Own Medicine: When the Doctor Is the Patient , 1988 .

[26]  George Loewenstein,et al.  A visceral account of addiction. , 2001 .

[27]  G. Sutherland,et al.  Will to live in the terminally ill , 1999 .

[28]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Wouldn't it be nice? Predicting future feelings. , 1999 .

[29]  M. Stroud Shadows on the wasteland : crossing Antarctica with Ranulph Fiennes , 1994 .

[30]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Obesity, Hunger, and Supermarket Shopping Behavior , 1968 .