The Suprachiasmatic Nucleus is not Required for Temporal Gating of Performance on a Reward-based Learning and Memory Task

In hamsters, the expression of a learned preference for context depends upon a temporal match between the time of training and testing. In the present experiments, we investigated the role of the biological clock in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) as a provider of temporal information underlying this time dependent modulation of cognitive performance. Hamsters were tested using the conditioned place preference task (CPP) before and after ablation of the SCN. Arrhythmic animals continued to show time-of-day modulation of the CPP when trained and tested in the absence of the SCN. This supports the notion that time of day information is a component of context representation for the hamster (Antoniadis et al., 1999), and indicates that an oscillator outside of the SCN is responsible for time discrimination in reward-based learning.

[1]  L. Kamin,et al.  The retention of an incompletely learned avoidance response. , 1957, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  Orfeu M Buxton,et al.  Locomotor response to an open field during C57BL/6J active and inactive phases differences dependent on conditions of illumination , 2000, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  F. Davis,et al.  Transplanted suprachiasmatic nucleus determines circadian period. , 1990, Science.

[4]  J. Bossert,et al.  Discrimination of circadian phase in intact and suprachiasmatic nuclei-ablated rats , 1996, Brain Research.

[5]  F. Stephan,et al.  Multiple retention deficit in passive avoidance in rats is eliminated by suprachiasmatic lesions. , 1978, Behavioral biology.

[6]  F. Holloway,et al.  Multiple retention deficits at periodic intervals after active and passive avoidance learning. , 1973, Behavioral biology.

[7]  M. Menaker,et al.  The tau mutation affects temperature compensation of hamster retinal circadian oscillators , 1998, Neuroreport.

[8]  Catalin V Buhusi,et al.  Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  Joel D Levine,et al.  Molecular Analysis of Mammalian Timeless , 1998, Neuron.

[10]  F. Holloway,et al.  Phase shifting circadian rhythms produces retrograde amnesia. , 1981, Science.

[11]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  No Time of Day Modulation or Time Stamp on Multiple Memory Tasks in Rats , 2002 .

[12]  L Hasher,et al.  Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  Kazuo Sato,et al.  Multitissue Circadian Expression of Rat periodHomolog (rPer2) mRNA Is Governed by the Mammalian Circadian Clock, the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus in the Brain* , 1998, The Journal of Biological Chemistry.

[14]  R. L. Mellgren,et al.  Multiple deficits in the retention of an appetitively motivated behavior across a 24-h period in rats , 1977 .

[15]  F. Holloway,et al.  Multiphasic Retention Deficits at Periodic Intervals after Passive-Avoidance Learning , 1973, Science.

[16]  J. Krebs,et al.  The Effect of Constant Light and Phase Shifts on a Learned Time-Place Association in Garden Warblers (Sylvia borin): Hourglass or Circadian Clock? , 1991, Journal of biological rhythms.

[17]  W H Meck,et al.  The 'internal clocks' of circadian and interval timing. , 1997, Endeavour.

[18]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Circadian Phase-Shifted Rats Show Normal Acquisition but Impaired Long-Term Retention of Place Information in the Water Task , 2001, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[19]  W. Meck,et al.  Dissecting the Brain's Internal Clock: How Frontal–Striatal Circuitry Keeps Time and Shifts Attention , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[20]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  The significance of circadian phase for performance on a reward-based learning task in hamsters , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[21]  M. Ralph,et al.  The Significance of Circadian Organization for Longevity in the Golden Hamster , 1998, Journal of biological rhythms.

[22]  G. Winocur,et al.  Aging and time-of-day effects on cognition in rats. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  Z. Boulos,et al.  Feeding schedules and the circadian organization of behavior in the rat , 1980, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  V. Navaratnam,et al.  THE EFFECT OF PHASE‐SHIFT ON THE PASSIVE AVOIDANCE RESPONSE IN RATS AND THE MODIFYING ACTION OF CHLORDIAZEPOXIDE , 1974, British journal of pharmacology.

[25]  S. Fairhurst,et al.  Scalar timing in animals and humans , 2002 .

[26]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Optimal Time of Day and the Magnitude of Age Differences in Memory , 1993 .

[27]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Circadian rhythms, aging and memory , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  T. Nagase,et al.  Antiphase circadian expression between BMAL1 and period homologue mRNA in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and peripheral tissues of rats. , 1998, Biochemical and biophysical research communications.

[29]  J. M. Ree,et al.  Disrupting circadian rhythms in rats induces retrograde amnesia , 1985, Physiology & Behavior.

[30]  T. Monk,et al.  Hours of work : temporal factors in work scheduling , 1985 .

[31]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  B. Rusak,et al.  Food-Anticipatory Circadian Rhythms in Rats with Paraventricular and Lateral Hypothalamic Ablations , 1988 .

[33]  F. Holloway,et al.  Multiple retention deficits following one-trial appetitive training. , 1975, Behavioral biology.

[34]  C. Sisk,et al.  Anticipation of 24-hr feeding schedules in rats with lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus. , 1979, Behavioral and neural biology.

[35]  J. L. Gould Honey bees store learned flower-landing behaviour according to time of day , 1987, Animal Behaviour.