Visual discriminations of spokes, sectors, and circles by the honeybee (Apis mellifera).

A new cue for visual discrimination by the honeybee has been demonstrated. Bees detected the position of the centre of symmetry of radial patterns of spokes, sectors, and circles relative to their point of choice in the learning process, irrespective of the pattern. When trained with one of these patterns versus a blank target, the bees discriminated a shift in the position of the centre of symmetry by as little as 5 degrees , in some cases with unfamiliar test patterns. A pattern of spokes or rings also stabilized the vision of the bees in the horizontal plane so that the position of a plain black area could then be discriminated. In other experiments, bees discriminated half of a pattern of radial spokes or concentric circles from the other half, cut either vertically or horizontally, and irrespective of scale. Therefore these patterns were not detected by preformed combinations of orientation detectors or global templates with a single output. Instead, the crucial cue for detecting edges as radial or circular was the coincidence of responses of numerous local edge detectors having the appropriate convergence to a hub. Edges that converged towards a hub were detected by the bees as radial, and edges at right angles to these were parts of circles, irrespective of the actual pattern. Breaking the patterns of spokes or circles into rows of squares spoiled the discrimination if the squares were separately resolved. Alternatively, breaking the pattern into short bars that were separately resolved spoiled the discrimination when the bars subtended less than 3 degrees . The local feature detectors for spokes and circles therefore resembled those of the orientation detectors in being short, independent, and unable to span gaps of more than 3 degrees . In conclusion, radial and circular patterns were identified by the regional coincidences and convergence of local detectors of edge orientation, and the positions of the centres of symmetry were remembered as landmarks that helped locate the reward, but the patterns themselves were not remembered.

[1]  K. Frisch Der Farbensinn und Formensinn der Biene , 1914 .

[2]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Visual Discrimination of Pattern Orientation by Honeybees: Performance and Implications for `Cortical' Processing , 1994 .

[3]  M. Hertz Die Organisation des optischen Feldes bei der Biene. I. , 2004, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[4]  George Adrian Horridge,et al.  Pattern vision in honeybees (Apis mellifera): Flower-like patterns with no predominant orientation , 1995 .

[5]  G. Horridge,et al.  Discrimination of single bars by the honeybee (Apis mellifera) , 2003, Vision Research.

[6]  R. Wehner Die Bedeutung der Streifenbreite für die optische Winkelmessung der Biene (Apis mellifica) , 1968, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[7]  Herbert Baumgärtner,et al.  Der Formensinn und die Sehschärfe der Bienen , 1928, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[8]  A. Horridge Visual discrimination by the honeybee (Apis mellifera): the position of the common centre as the cue , 2003 .

[9]  Horridge Visual discrimination of radial cues by the honeybee (Apis mellifera). , 2000, Journal of insect physiology.

[10]  Perception of edges versus areas by the crab Carcinus. , 1966, The Journal of experimental biology.

[11]  H. Cruse Versuch einer quantitativen Beschreibung des Formensehens der Honigbiene , 1972, Kybernetik.

[12]  Z. Kaprielian,et al.  The molecular basis of retinotectal topography , 1994, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[13]  A. Horridge Pattern Vision of the Honeybee (Apis mellifera): Blue and Green Receptors in the Discrimination of Translocation , 2000, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[14]  Adrian Horridge,et al.  The spatial resolutions of the apposition compound eye and its neuro-sensory feature detectors: observation versus theory. , 2005, Journal of insect physiology.

[15]  Adrian Horridge,et al.  Bee vision of pattern and 3D. The Bidder Lecture 1994 , 1994 .

[16]  A Horridge Seven experiments on pattern vision of the honeybee, with a model , 2000, Vision Research.

[17]  Adrian Horridge The visual system of the honeybee (Apis mellifera): the maximum length of the orientation detector. , 2003, Journal of insect physiology.

[18]  G. Horridge Pattern discrimination by the honeybee (Apis mellifera ) is colour blind for radial / tangential cues , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[19]  Alun M. Anderson A model for landmark learning in the honey-bee , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[20]  R. Wehner Spatial Vision in Arthropods , 1981 .

[21]  George Adrian Horridge,et al.  Shape vision in bees: innate preference for flower-like patterns , 1995 .

[22]  M. Friedlaender Zur Bedeutung des Fluglochs im optischen Feld der Biene bei senkrechter Dressuranordnung , 1931, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[23]  A. Horridge Pattern vision of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). What is an oriented edge? , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[24]  J. L. Gould How Bees Remember Flower Shapes , 1985, Science.

[25]  Pattern vision of the honeybee (Apis mellifera): the effect of pattern on the discrimination of location , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[26]  Adrian Horridge Visual resolution of the orientation cue by the honeybee (Apis mellifera). , 2003, Journal of insect physiology.

[27]  G. Horridge,et al.  Vision of the honeybee Apis mellifera for patterns with one pair of equal orthogonal bars. , 1997, Journal of insect physiology.

[28]  A. Horridge What the honeybee sees: a review of the recognition system of Apis mellifera , 2005 .