Let's Move It Together: A Review of Group Benefits in Joint Object Control

In daily life, humans frequently engage in object-directed joint actions, be it carrying a table together or jointly pulling a rope. When two or more individuals control an object together, they may distribute control by performing complementary actions, e.g., when two people hold a table at opposite ends. Alternatively, several individuals may execute control in a redundant manner by performing the same actions, e.g., when jointly pulling a rope in the same direction. Previous research has investigated whether dyads can outperform individuals in tasks where control is either distributed or redundant. The aim of the present review is to integrate findings for these two types of joint control to determine common principles and explain differing results. In sum, we find that when control is distributed, individuals tend to outperform dyads or attain similar performance levels. For redundant control, conversely, dyads have been shown to outperform individuals. We suggest that these differences can be explained by the possibility to freely divide control: Having the option to exercise control redundantly allows co-actors to coordinate individual contributions in line with individual capabilities, enabling them to maximize the benefit of the available skills in the group. In contrast, this freedom to adopt and adapt customized coordination strategies is not available when the distribution of control is determined from the outset.

[1]  Peter König,et al.  Benefiting from Being Alike: Interindividual Skill Differences Predict Collective Benefit in Joint Object Control , 2019, CogSci.

[2]  P. König,et al.  Group benefits in joint perceptual tasks—a review , 2018, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  P. König,et al.  Performance similarities predict collective benefits in dyadic and triadic joint visual search , 2018, PloS one.

[4]  Marco Santello,et al.  On the Role of Physical Interaction on Performance of Object Manipulation by Dyads , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  P. König,et al.  Two Trackers Are Better than One: Information about the Co-actor's Actions and Performance Scores Contribute to the Collective Benefit in a Joint Visuospatial Task , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[6]  Junya Masumoto,et al.  Bidirectional transfer between joint and individual actions in a task of discrete force production , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Physically interacting individuals estimate the partner’s goal to enhance their movements , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[8]  Sari R. R. Nijssen,et al.  Joint Action: Mental Representations, Shared Information and General Mechanisms for Coordinating with Others , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[9]  J. Enns,et al.  What’s in a Friendship? Partner Visibility Supports Cognitive Collaboration between Friends , 2015, PloS one.

[10]  J. Enns,et al.  When two heads are better than one: Interactive versus independent benefits of collaborative cognition , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  Nobuyuki Inui,et al.  Motor control hierarchy in joint action that involves bimanual force production. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Junya Masumoto,et al.  A leader–follower relationship in joint action on a discrete force production task , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  M. Kawato,et al.  Two is better than one: Physical interactions improve motor performance in humans , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[14]  Junya Masumoto,et al.  Two heads are better than one: both complementary and synchronous strategies facilitate joint action. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Natalie Sebanz,et al.  The sense of agency during skill learning in individuals and dyads , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[16]  Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al.  Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Kyle B. Reed,et al.  Physical Collaboration of Human-Human and Human-Robot Teams , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[18]  Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al.  Anatomical substrates of cooperative joint-action in a continuous motor task: Virtual lifting and balancing , 2008, NeuroImage.

[19]  Jurjen Bosga,et al.  Joint-action coordination of redundant force contributions in a virtual lifting task. , 2007, Motor control.

[20]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Haptically Linked Dyads , 2006, Psychological science.

[21]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  G. Knoblich,et al.  Action coordination in groups and individuals: learning anticipatory control. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[24]  Edwin Hutchins,et al.  How a Cockpit Remembers Its Speeds , 1995, Cogn. Sci..

[25]  H. C. Bolton I.—Application of Organic Acids to the Examination of Minerals. (With Plate I.) , 1879 .