Let's Move It Together: A Review of Group Benefits in Joint Object Control
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Peter König,et al. Benefiting from Being Alike: Interindividual Skill Differences Predict Collective Benefit in Joint Object Control , 2019, CogSci.
[2] P. König,et al. Group benefits in joint perceptual tasks—a review , 2018, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[3] P. König,et al. Performance similarities predict collective benefits in dyadic and triadic joint visual search , 2018, PloS one.
[4] Marco Santello,et al. On the Role of Physical Interaction on Performance of Object Manipulation by Dyads , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[5] P. König,et al. Two Trackers Are Better than One: Information about the Co-actor's Actions and Performance Scores Contribute to the Collective Benefit in a Joint Visuospatial Task , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[6] Junya Masumoto,et al. Bidirectional transfer between joint and individual actions in a task of discrete force production , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Physically interacting individuals estimate the partner’s goal to enhance their movements , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[8] Sari R. R. Nijssen,et al. Joint Action: Mental Representations, Shared Information and General Mechanisms for Coordinating with Others , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[9] J. Enns,et al. What’s in a Friendship? Partner Visibility Supports Cognitive Collaboration between Friends , 2015, PloS one.
[10] J. Enns,et al. When two heads are better than one: Interactive versus independent benefits of collaborative cognition , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[11] Nobuyuki Inui,et al. Motor control hierarchy in joint action that involves bimanual force production. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] Junya Masumoto,et al. A leader–follower relationship in joint action on a discrete force production task , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] M. Kawato,et al. Two is better than one: Physical interactions improve motor performance in humans , 2014, Scientific Reports.
[14] Junya Masumoto,et al. Two heads are better than one: both complementary and synchronous strategies facilitate joint action. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] Natalie Sebanz,et al. The sense of agency during skill learning in individuals and dyads , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.
[16] Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al. Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] Kyle B. Reed,et al. Physical Collaboration of Human-Human and Human-Robot Teams , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.
[18] Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al. Anatomical substrates of cooperative joint-action in a continuous motor task: Virtual lifting and balancing , 2008, NeuroImage.
[19] Jurjen Bosga,et al. Joint-action coordination of redundant force contributions in a virtual lifting task. , 2007, Motor control.
[20] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Haptically Linked Dyads , 2006, Psychological science.
[21] H. Bekkering,et al. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[22] G. Knoblich,et al. Action coordination in groups and individuals: learning anticipatory control. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[23] K. Doya,et al. A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[24] Edwin Hutchins,et al. How a Cockpit Remembers Its Speeds , 1995, Cogn. Sci..
[25] H. C. Bolton. I.—Application of Organic Acids to the Examination of Minerals. (With Plate I.) , 1879 .