The exploratory behavior of rats in an open environment optimizes security

When given a locomotor/exploratory test in the laboratory, rats form one or more home bases, operationally defined as places where they spend a disproportionate period of their time and from which they make excursions. Because exploratory tests in the laboratory necessarily restrict the animals' movements, the cause of exploration (e.g., fear, curiosity, innate disposition) and the extent to which organization is imposed by the restriction of the testing environment has not been fully examined. In the present study, rats received exploratory tests in environments in which restrictions were remote; in a parking lot or on a playing field. Each rat began a test in one of three conditions: in a small refuge, within a transparent open home cage, or beside a landmark. In the parking lot, the rats failed to leave the small refuge, made excursions from the home cage, and left the landmark, usually at a gallop, and made no movements of returning. On the playing field they remained in the small refuge, left and returned to the open home cage, and were more likely to permanently leave the landmark at a gallop. Rats that displayed a strong preference for the landmark over three test sessions in a laboratory, also immediately left the same landmark when tested on the playing field. The pattern of behavior, in which the rats failed to explore from a secure starting position and were increasingly likely to run away as security decreased, suggests that a primary function of locomotor behavior in a novel environment is to optimize security. The results are discussed in relation to the advantages of investigating the influence of neural processes on exploration in terms optimization theory versus motivational theory.

[1]  L. Weiskrantz Analysis of behavioral change , 1968 .

[2]  D. Mumby,et al.  Hippocampal damage and exploratory preferences in rats: memory for objects, places, and contexts. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[3]  F. Graeff,et al.  Ethopharmacological analysis of rat behavior on the elevated plus-maze , 1994, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[4]  Yoav Benjamini,et al.  Statistical discrimination of natural modes of motion in rat exploratory behavior , 2000, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[5]  J. Siegel,et al.  Exploratory Behavior as a Function of Environmental Novelty and Complexity in Male and Female Rats , 2005, Psychological reports.

[6]  Benjamin J. Clark,et al.  Movements of exploration intact in rats with hippocampal lesions , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  Erik Woody,et al.  Obsessive-compulsive disorder as a disturbance of security motivation. , 2004, Psychological review.

[8]  B. Meyerson,et al.  The Concentric Square Field: A multivariate test arena for analysis of explorative strategies , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  D. M. Skinner,et al.  The Moving Fire Hydrant Experiment: Movement of Objects to a New Location Reelicits Marking in Rats , 2001, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[10]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Locomotor and Exploratory Behavior , 2004 .

[11]  D. Mumby,et al.  Rat (Rattus norvegicus) defensive behavior in total darkness: risk-assessment function of defensive burying. , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[12]  T. Kosten,et al.  Strain differences in response to escapable and inescapable novel environments and their ability to predict amphetamine-induced locomotor activity , 2003, Psychopharmacology.

[13]  C. Kirstein,et al.  Effects of novelty on behavior in the adolescent and adult rat. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[14]  L. Stefanacci,et al.  Bright light suppresses hyperactivity induced by excitotoxic dorsal hippocampus lesions in the rat. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  F. Holsboer,et al.  Behavioural profiles of two Wistar rat lines selectively bred for high or low anxiety-related behaviour , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  S. Henriksen,et al.  Electrophysiological responses of nucleus accumbens neurons to novelty stimuli and exploratory behavior in the awake, unrestrained rat , 1998, Brain Research.

[17]  G. P. Smith,et al.  Mesolimbic and mesocortical dopaminergic neurons are necessary for normal exploratory behavior in rats , 1980, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  C. S. Hall,et al.  Emotional behavior in the rat. I. Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality. , 1934 .

[19]  D. Eilam,et al.  Home base behavior of rats (Rattus norvegicus) exploring a novel environment , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Ofer Tchernichovski,et al.  The dynamics of long-term exploration in the rat , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Home bases formed to visual cues but not to self‐movement (dead reckoning) cues in exploring hippocampectomized rats , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  C. H. Vanderwolf,et al.  p-Chlorophenylalanine-induced serotonin depletion: reduction in exploratory locomotion but no obvious sensory-motor deficits , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[23]  D. O. Hebb,et al.  The organization of behavior , 1988 .

[24]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Defensive reactions and exploratory behavior in rats. , 1974 .

[25]  Klaus-Peter Ossenkopp,et al.  Measuring Movement and Locomotion: From Invertebrates to Humans , 1996 .

[26]  D. Eilam,et al.  Psychostimulant-Induced Behavior as an Animal Model of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: An Ethological Approach to the Form of Compulsive Rituals , 2005, CNS Spectrums.

[27]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Rats (Rattus norvegicus) modulate eating speed and vigilance to optimize food consumption: effects of cover, circadian rhythm, food deprivation, and individual differences. , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[28]  T. Schallert,et al.  CNS plasticity and assessment of forelimb sensorimotor outcome in unilateral rat models of stroke, cortical ablation, parkinsonism and spinal cord injury , 2000, Neuropharmacology.

[29]  J. B. Calhoun The ecology and sociology of the Norway rat. , 1963 .

[30]  H. Arakawa Changes in the pattern of exploratory behavior are associated with the emergence of social dominance relationships in male rats. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[31]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[32]  I. Whishaw,et al.  The development of spatial capacity in piloting and dead reckoning by infant rats: use of the huddle as a home base for spatial navigation. , 2005, Developmental psychobiology.

[33]  K. Montgomery,et al.  The relation between fear and exploratory behavior. , 1955, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Conspecific aggression influences food carrying : studies on a wild population of Rattus norvegicus , 1996 .

[35]  E. M. Lee,et al.  Movement arrest and the hippocampus , 1977 .

[36]  I. Whishaw,et al.  NMDA lesions of Ammon's horn and the dentate gyrus disrupt the direct and temporally paced homing displayed by rats exploring a novel environment: evidence for a role of the hippocampus in dead reckoning , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  G. Elmer,et al.  Texture of locomotor path: a replicable characterization of a complex behavioral phenotype , 2005, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[38]  David Eilam,et al.  Stopping behavior: constraints on exploration in rats (Rattus norvegicus) , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  P. Russell,et al.  Relationships between exploratory behaviour and fear: a review. , 1973, British journal of psychology.

[40]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Dissociating context and space within the hippocampus: effects of complete, dorsal, and ventral excitotoxic hippocampal lesions on conditioned freezing and spatial learning. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  D C Blanchard,et al.  Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[42]  Ian Q. Whishaw,et al.  The topography of three-dimensional exploration: a new quantification of vertical and horizontal exploration, postural support, and exploratory bouts in the cylinder test , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[43]  Chun-I Li,et al.  Cue and context conditioning of defensive behaviors to cat odor stimuli , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[44]  R. Hinde,et al.  Biological Bases Of Human Social Behaviour , 1975 .

[45]  D. Blizard,et al.  Sex differences in open-field behavior in the rat: The inductive and activational role of gonadal hormones , 1975, Physiology & Behavior.

[46]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Dead reckoning (path integration) requires the hippocampal formation: evidence from spontaneous exploration and spatial learning tasks in light (allothetic) and dark (idiothetic) tests , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[47]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  The Ecology of Fear: Optimal Foraging, Game Theory, and Trophic Interactions , 1999 .

[48]  K. Montgomery,et al.  The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory drive. , 1955 .

[49]  I. Whishaw,et al.  The role of D1 and D2 receptors in the heightened locomotion induced by direct and indirect dopamine agonists in rats with hippocampal damage: An animal analogue of schizophrenia , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[50]  M. J. Renner,et al.  Molar characteristics of exploratory and investigatory behavior in the rat (Rattus norvegicus). , 1991, Journal of comparative psychology.

[51]  I. Weiner,et al.  Rat Strain Differences in Open-Field Behavior and the Locomotor Stimulating and Rewarding Effects of Amphetamine , 1998, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[52]  N. Canteras,et al.  An alternative experimental procedure for studying predator-related defensive responses , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.