Auditory-nerve responses predict pitch attributes related to musical consonance-dissonance for normal and impaired hearing.

Human listeners prefer consonant over dissonant musical intervals and the perceived contrast between these classes is reduced with cochlear hearing loss. Population-level activity of normal and impaired model auditory-nerve (AN) fibers was examined to determine (1) if peripheral auditory neurons exhibit correlates of consonance and dissonance and (2) if the reduced perceptual difference between these qualities observed for hearing-impaired listeners can be explained by impaired AN responses. In addition, acoustical correlates of consonance-dissonance were also explored including periodicity and roughness. Among the chromatic pitch combinations of music, consonant intervals/chords yielded more robust neural pitch-salience magnitudes (determined by harmonicity/periodicity) than dissonant intervals/chords. In addition, AN pitch-salience magnitudes correctly predicted the ordering of hierarchical pitch and chordal sonorities described by Western music theory. Cochlear hearing impairment compressed pitch salience estimates between consonant and dissonant pitch relationships. The reduction in contrast of neural responses following cochlear hearing loss may explain the inability of hearing-impaired listeners to distinguish musical qualia as clearly as normal-hearing individuals. Of the neural and acoustic correlates explored, AN pitch salience was the best predictor of behavioral data. Results ultimately show that basic pitch relationships governing music are already present in initial stages of neural processing at the AN level.

[1]  E. M. Burns Intervals, Scales, and Tuning , 1999 .

[2]  Gavin M. Bidelman,et al.  Neural Correlates of Consonance, Dissonance, and the Hierarchy of Musical Pitch in the Human Brainstem , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  I. Peretz,et al.  Musical scale properties are automatically processed in the human auditory cortex , 2006, Brain Research.

[4]  Tsutomu Nakada,et al.  Central auditory processing of noncontextual consonance in music: an evoked potential study. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  Vesa Välimäki,et al.  Neural representations of the hierarchical scale pitch structure , 2007 .

[6]  E. F. Evans,et al.  Some aspects of temporal coding by single cochlear fibres from regions of cochlear hair cell degeneration in the guinea pig , 2004, Archives of oto-rhino-laryngology.

[7]  E D Young,et al.  Rate responses of auditory nerve fibers to tones in noise near masked threshold. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Michael G. Heinz,et al.  Envelope Coding in Auditory Nerve Fibers Following Noise-Induced Hearing Loss , 2010, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.

[9]  S. Watanabe,et al.  Discrimination of consonance and dissonance in Java sparrows , 2005, Behavioural Processes.

[10]  R. Patterson,et al.  The lower limit of melodic pitch. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  Lewi Stone,et al.  Perception of musical consonance and dissonance: an outcome of neural synchronization , 2008, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[12]  G. K. Yates,et al.  Outer hair cell receptor current and sensorineural hearing loss , 1989, Hearing Research.

[13]  Brian C. J. Moore,et al.  Perception of Pitch by People with Cochlear Hearing Loss and by Cochlear Implant Users , 2005 .

[14]  Gavin M Bidelman,et al.  Brainstem correlates of behavioral and compositional preferences of musical harmony , 2011, Neuroreport.

[15]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neurobiological Foundations for the Theory of Harmony in Western Tonal Music , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  R. Plomp,et al.  Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  C E Schreiner,et al.  Neural processing of amplitude-modulated sounds. , 2004, Physiological reviews.

[18]  A comparison of monotic and dichotic complex-tone pitch perception in listeners with hearing loss. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  W. Sethares Local consonance and the relationship between timbre and scale , 1993 .

[20]  M. Liberman,et al.  Single-neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology. III. Stereocilia damage and alterations of threshold tuning curves , 1984, Hearing Research.

[21]  K. James,et al.  Neural correlates of the Pythagorean ratio rules , 2007, Neuroreport.

[22]  Martin Ebeling,et al.  Neuronal periodicity detection as a basis for the perception of consonance: a mathematical model of tonal fusion. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  Josh H. McDermott,et al.  Nonhuman primates prefer slow tempos but dislike music overall , 2007, Cognition.

[24]  E. Terhardt On the perception of periodic sound fluctuations (roughness) , 1974 .

[25]  Frank H. Slaymaker,et al.  Chords from Tones Having Stretched Partials , 1970 .

[26]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Pitch of complex tones: rate-place and interspike interval representations in the auditory nerve. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  P. Cariani,et al.  A TEMPORAL MODEL FOR PITCH MULTIPLICITY AND TONAL CONSONANCE , 2004 .

[28]  Hideko Takeshita,et al.  Preference for consonant music over dissonant music by an infant chimpanzee , 2009, Primates.

[29]  Ludovico Minati,et al.  Functional MRI/Event-related potential study of sensory consonance and dissonance in musicians and nonmusicians , 2009, Neuroreport.

[30]  Laurel H Carney,et al.  A phenomenological model of the synapse between the inner hair cell and auditory nerve: long-term adaptation with power-law dynamics. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Gavin M. Bidelman,et al.  Cross-domain Effects of Music and Language Experience on the Representation of Pitch in the Human Auditory Brainstem , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Kameoka,et al.  Consonance theory part I: consonance of dyads. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  Daniel I. Brooks,et al.  Chord Discrimination by Pigeons , 2010 .

[34]  Vit Drga,et al.  Inferred basilar-membrane response functions for listeners with mild to moderate sensorineural hearing loss. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  M. Sachs,et al.  An auditory-periphery model of the effects of acoustic trauma on auditory nerve responses. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  S Shamma,et al.  The case of the missing pitch templates: how harmonic templates emerge in the early auditory system. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[37]  A. Izumi,et al.  Japanese monkeys perceive sensory consonance of chords. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[38]  Marc Hauser,et al.  Are consonant intervals music to their ears? Spontaneous acoustic preferences in a nonhuman primate , 2004, Cognition.

[39]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Pitch Representations in the Auditory Nerve: Two Concurrent Complex Tones Chair, Department Committee on Graduate Students , 2022 .

[40]  A. Ryan,et al.  Neural phase-locking properties in the absence of cochlear outer hair cells , 1981, Hearing Research.

[41]  M Steinschneider,et al.  Consonance and dissonance of musical chords: neural correlates in auditory cortex of monkeys and humans. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Andrew J Oxenham,et al.  An autocorrelation model with place dependence to account for the effect of harmonic number on fundamental frequency discrimination. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[43]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Tonal fusion of consonant musical intervals: The oomph in Stumpf , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  Jayaganesh Swaminathan,et al.  Across-Fiber Coding of Temporal Fine-Structure: Effects of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss on Auditory-Nerve Responses , 2010 .

[45]  Brian C. J. Moore,et al.  Effect of loudness recruitment on the perception of amplitude modulation , 1996 .

[46]  Josh H. McDermott,et al.  Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance , 2010, Current Biology.

[47]  E. Lopez-Poveda,et al.  The neurophysiological bases of auditory perception , 2010 .

[48]  R. Plomp,et al.  The connotation of musical consonance , 1962 .

[49]  Josh H. McDermott,et al.  THE ORIGINS OF MUSIC: INNATENESS, UNIQUENESS, AND EVOLUTION , 2005 .

[50]  Paul C. Boomsliter,et al.  The Long Pattern Hypothesis in Harmony and Hearing , 1961 .

[51]  Michelle R. Molis,et al.  Perception of roughness by listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. , 2007, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[52]  M. Leek,et al.  Pitch strength and pitch dominance of iterated rippled noises in hearing-impaired listeners. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[53]  Takashi X. Fujisawa,et al.  The Psychophysics of Harmony Perception: Harmony is a Three-Tone Phenomenon , 2006 .

[54]  James Tenney,et al.  A history of consonance and dissonance , 1988 .

[55]  Muhammad S A Zilany,et al.  Modeling auditory-nerve responses for high sound pressure levels in the normal and impaired auditory periphery. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[56]  S. Trehub,et al.  Infant music perception: Domain-general or domain-specific mechanisms? , 2006, Cognition.

[57]  Christopher A Shera,et al.  Revised estimates of human cochlear tuning from otoacoustic and behavioral measurements , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  Ian C. Bruce,et al.  Representation of the vowel /ε/ in normal and impaired auditory nerve fibers: Model predictions of responses in cats , 2007 .

[59]  Michelle R. Molis,et al.  Perception of dissonance by people with normal hearing and sensorineural hearing loss. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[60]  Hermann von Helmholtz,et al.  On the Sensations of Tone , 1954 .

[61]  A. Kameoka,et al.  Consonance theory part II: consonance of complex tones and its calculation method. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[62]  E. Terhardt,et al.  Algorithm for extraction of pitch and pitch salience from complex tonal signals , 1982 .

[63]  D. Purves,et al.  A Biological Rationale for Musical Scales , 2009, PloS one.

[64]  P. Vassilakis,et al.  Auditory roughness as means of musical expression , 2005 .

[65]  D. Purves,et al.  The Statistical Structure of Human Speech Sounds Predicts Musical Universals , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  J. M. Troost,et al.  Ascending and Descending Melodic Intervals: Statistical Findings and Their Perceptual Relevance , 1989 .

[67]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neural correlates of the pitch of complex tones. I. Pitch and pitch salience. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[68]  J. L. Goldstein An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[69]  E D Young,et al.  Effects of acoustic trauma on the representation of the vowel "eh" in cat auditory nerve fibers. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.