Attribution in conversational context: Effect of mutual knowledge on explanation‐giving

Attribution theorists typically have conceived the attribution process in terms of universal laws of cognitive functioning, independent of social interaction. In this paper we argue for the notion, grounded in recent ordinary language philosophy, that any consideration of the form of everyday explanation must take into account its function as an answer to a ‘why’ question within a conversational framework. Experiment 1 provides support for the idea that speakers should identify as causally relevant that necessary condition for the occurrence of an event about which the enquirer is ignorant. Experiment 2 replicates this basic finding and further demonstrates that speakers will change their explanations to enquirers believed to be sharing different knowledge about the same target event. Experiment 2 also assessed the role of individual differences in conversational rule-following, and found in apparent contrast some previous predictions that high self-monitoring individuals were no more likely than lows to tailor their explanations to suit the enquirer's knowledge state. If anything, the reverse occurred. Taken together, these experiments support the central contention of the abnormal conditions focus model (Hilton and Slugoski, 1986), that the common sense criterion of causality is that of an ‘abnormal condition’ rather than constant conjunction as instantiated in the ANOVA model of causal attribution (Kelley, 1967, 1973).

[1]  W. Turnbull Everyday Explanation: The Pragmatics of Puzzle Resolution , 1986 .

[2]  James M. Dabbs,et al.  Self-Monitors in Conversation: What Do They Monitor?. , 1980 .

[3]  H. Kelley The processes of causal attribution. , 1973 .

[4]  Teresa M. Amabile,et al.  Social Roles, Social Control and Biases in Social Perception , 1977 .

[5]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .

[6]  M. Snyder,et al.  Persons, situations, and the control of social behavior. , 1975 .

[7]  A. L. McGill,et al.  Context effects in judgments of causation. , 1989 .

[8]  Bruce R. Orvis,et al.  A closer examination of causal inference: The roles of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. , 1975 .

[9]  L. A. McArthur The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. , 1972 .

[10]  P. White A Model of the Layperson as Pragmatist , 1984 .

[11]  E. E. Jones,et al.  From Acts To Dispositions The Attribution Process In Person Perception1 , 1965 .

[12]  D. Hilton,et al.  Knowledge-Based Causal Attribution: The Abnormal Conditions Focus Model , 1986 .

[13]  E. Higgins,et al.  The “Communication Game”: Goal-Directed Encoding and Cognitive Consequences , 1982 .

[14]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Primacy and recency in communication and self-persuasion : how successive audiences and multiple encodings influence subsequent evaluative judgments , 1991 .

[15]  A. Koller,et al.  Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language , 1969 .

[16]  V. A. Harris,et al.  The Attribution of Attitudes , 1967 .

[17]  C. McCann,et al.  Self-monitoring in communicative interactions: Social cognitive consequences of goal-directed message modification , 1983 .

[18]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1997 .

[19]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  "Saying is believing": Effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described. , 1978 .

[20]  D. Byrne Interpersonal attraction and attitude similarity. , 1961, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[21]  William Ickes,et al.  The Role of Sex and Self-Monitoring in Unstructured Dyadic Interactions , 1977 .

[22]  R. Lippa Expressive control and the leakage of dispositional introversion-extraversion during role-played teaching. , 1976, Journal of personality.

[23]  Penelope Brown,et al.  Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage , 1989 .

[24]  D. Hilton Conversational processes and causal explanation. , 1990 .

[25]  J. Earman,et al.  The Cement Of The Universe , 1974 .

[26]  A gloss on attribution theory , 1980 .

[27]  M. Snyder Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. , 1974 .

[28]  Daniel M. Wegner,et al.  Where Leading Questions Can Lead: The Power of Conjecture in Social Interaction , 1982 .

[29]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  The accountability of conduct : a social psychological analysis , 1984 .

[30]  B. Slugoski,et al.  Cruel to be Kind and Kind to be Cruel: Sarcasm, Banter and Social Relations , 1988 .

[31]  L. Ross,et al.  The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process”, in Ed), Advances in Experimental Social New York, pp. . , 1977 .

[32]  G. Elliott Some Effects of Deception and Level of Self-Monitoring on Planning and Reacting to a Self-Presentation , 1979 .

[33]  G. W. Bradley Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. , 1978 .