Boredom-proneness, loneliness, social engagement and depression and their association with cognitive function in older people: A population study

In this study, we use data from a population survey of persons aged 65 and over living in the Irish Republic to examine the relationship of cognitive impairment, assessed using the Abbreviated Mental Test, with loneliness, boredom-proneness, social relations, and depression. Participants were randomly selected community-dwelling Irish people aged 65+ years. An Abbreviated Mental Test score of 8 or 9 out of 10 was classified as ‘low normal’, and a score of less than 8 as ‘possible cognitive impairment’. We used clustering around latent variables analysis (CLV) to identify families of variables associated with reduced cognitive function. The overall prevalence of possible cognitive impairment was 14.7% (95% CI 12.4–17.3%). Low normal scores had a prevalence of 30.5% (95% CI 27.2–33.7%). CLV analysis identified three groups of predictors: ‘Low social support’ (widowed, living alone, low social support), ‘personal cognitive reserve’ (low social activity, no leisure exercise, never having married, loneliness and boredom-proneness), and ‘sociodemographic cognitive reserve’ (primary education, rural domicile). In multivariate analysis, both cognitive reserve clusters, but not social support, were independently associated with cognitive function. Loneliness and boredom-proneness are associated with reduced cognitive function in older age, and cluster with other factors associated with cognitive reserve. Both may have a common underlying mechanism in the failure to select and maintain attention on particular features of the social environment (loneliness) or the non-social environment (boredom-proneness).

[1]  G. Huston The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. , 1987, The Journal of rheumatology.

[2]  S. Vodanovich,et al.  Boredom proneness: its relationship to psychological- and physical-health symptoms. , 2000, Journal of clinical psychology.

[3]  John S. Allen,et al.  Normal neuroanatomical variation due to age: The major lobes and a parcellation of the temporal region , 2005, Neurobiology of Aging.

[4]  Brian A. Lawlor,et al.  Social support network structure in older people: Underlying dimensions and association with psychological and physical health , 2009, Psychology, health & medicine.

[5]  Laura Fratiglioni,et al.  Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. , 2002, American journal of epidemiology.

[6]  H. Hodkinson Evaluation of a mental test score for assessment of mental impairment in the elderly. , 1972, Age and ageing.

[7]  D M MacKenzie,et al.  Brief cognitive screening of the elderly: a comparison of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Abbreviated Mental Test (AMT) and Mental Status Questionnaire (MSQ) , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[8]  C. Chew‐Graham,et al.  Validating screening instruments for cognitive impairment in older South Asians in the United Kingdom , 2000, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[9]  T. T. Haug,et al.  The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. An updated literature review. , 2002, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[10]  R. Jacoby,et al.  Cognitive impairment in medical inpatients. I: Screening for dementia--is history better than mental state? , 1997, Age and ageing.

[11]  T. A. Pychyl,et al.  Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: State orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom , 1998 .

[12]  J. J. Morales-Virgen [The epidemiology of dementias]. , 1997, Gaceta medica de Mexico.

[13]  E. Vigneau,et al.  Clustering of Variables Around Latent Components , 2003 .

[14]  Michael Kirby,et al.  Loneliness, social support networks, mood and wellbeing in community‐dwelling elderly , 2009, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[15]  M. J. Blanchard,et al.  Boredom Proneness and the Need for Cognition , 1994 .

[16]  George W Rebok,et al.  Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[17]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[18]  T. Strandberg,et al.  Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[19]  François Béland,et al.  Trajectories of cognitive decline and social relations. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[20]  Maxine Weinstein,et al.  Participating in social activities helps preserve cognitive function: an analysis of a longitudinal, population-based study of the elderly. , 2005, International journal of epidemiology.

[21]  B. Winblad,et al.  Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study , 2000, The Lancet.

[22]  Hui-Xin Wang,et al.  Brain reserve hypothesis in dementia. , 2007, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[23]  D. Bennett,et al.  Cognitive leisure activities, but not watching TV, for future brain benefits , 2006, Neurology.

[24]  C. Chew‐Graham,et al.  Screening for cognitive impairment in older African-Caribbeans , 2000, Psychological Medicine.

[25]  Laura Fratiglioni,et al.  The epidemiology of the dementias: an update , 2007, Current opinion in psychiatry.

[26]  A. Otero,et al.  Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[27]  D. G. Swain,et al.  Cognitive assessment in elderly patients admitted to hospital: the relationship between the Abbreviated Mental Test and the Mini-Mental State Examination , 1999, Clinical rehabilitation.

[28]  D. O’Neill,et al.  One Island - Two Systems , 2005 .

[29]  H. Damasio,et al.  The aging brain: The cognitive reserve hypothesis and hominid evolution , 2005, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[30]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: A subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[31]  H M Hodkinson,et al.  Evaluation of a mental test score for assessment of mental impairment in the elderly. 1972. , 2012, Age and ageing.

[32]  M. Chen,et al.  Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: The Chongqing aging study , 2006, Neurology.

[33]  H. McGee,et al.  One Island – Two Systems: A comparison of health status and health and social service use by community-dwelling older people in the Republic of Ireland and Northern Ireland , 2005 .

[34]  B. Winblad,et al.  An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia , 2004, The Lancet Neurology.

[35]  S. Vodanovich,et al.  Cognitive correlates of boredom proneness: the role of private self-consciousness and absorption. , 1998, The Journal of psychology.

[36]  D. Blazer,et al.  Self-efficacy and depression in late life: A primary prevention proposal , 2002, Aging & mental health.

[37]  A. L. de Munain,et al.  The validity of Hodkinson’s Abbreviated Mental Test for dementia screening in Guipuzcoa, Spain , 2001, European journal of neurology.

[38]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease , 1993, Neurology.

[39]  J. Schneider,et al.  The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer's disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study , 2006, The Lancet Neurology.

[40]  J. Schneider,et al.  Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.