Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems

In this study, 100 college students compared their own chances of experiencing 45 different health- and life-threatening problems with the chances of their peers. They showed a significant optimistic bias for 34 of these hazards, consistently considering their own chances to be below average. Attempts to account for the amount of bias evoked by different hazards identified perceived controllability, lack of previous experience, and the belief that the problem appears during childhood as factors that tend to increase unrealistic optimism. The investigation also examined the importance of beliefs and emotions as determinants of self-reported interest in adopting precautions to reduce one's risk. It found that: (a) beliefs about risk likelihood, beliefs about risk severity, and worry about the risk all made independent contributions to interest in risk reduction; (b) unrealistic optimism undermined interest in risk reduction indirectly by decreasing worry; and (c) beliefs about risk likelihood and severity were not sufficient to explain the amount of worry expressed about different hazards.

[1]  L. Ross,et al.  The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes , 1977 .

[2]  O. Svenson ARE WE ALL LESS RISKY AND MORE SKILLFUL THAN OUR FELLOW DRIVERS , 1981 .

[3]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Findings and Theory in the Study of Fear Communications , 1970 .

[4]  E. E. Jones,et al.  The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. , 1972 .

[5]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Attribution of success and failure revisited, or: The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory , 1979 .

[6]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Changing attitudes and habits to reduce risk factors in chronic disease. , 1973, The American journal of cardiology.

[7]  I. Rosenstock,et al.  A national study of health beliefs. , 1966, Journal of health and human behavior.

[8]  I. Rosenstock,et al.  Some influences on public participation in a genetic screening program , 2005, Journal of Community Health.

[9]  Irving L. Janis,et al.  Effects of Fear Arousal on Attitude Change: Recent Developments in Theory and Experimental Research1 , 1967 .

[10]  K. Higbee,et al.  Fifteen years of fear arousal: research on threat appeals: 1953-1968. , 1969, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  I. Rosenstock Historical Origins of the Health Belief Model , 1974 .

[12]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: Origins and Correlates in Psychological Theory* , 1974 .

[13]  D. Harris,et al.  Health-protective behavior: an exploratory study. , 1979, Journal of health and social behavior.

[14]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[15]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction? , 1975 .

[16]  R. Lazarus Psychological stress and the coping process , 1970 .

[17]  R W Rogers,et al.  Fear appeals and attitude change: effects of a threat's noxiousness, probability of occurrence, and the efficacy of coping responses. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  A. Jette,et al.  Psychosocial Determinants of Immunization Behavior in a Swine Influenza Campaign , 1979, Medical care.

[19]  Laurie Larwood,et al.  Swine Flu: A Field Study of Self-Serving Biases , 1978 .

[20]  G. W. Bradley Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. , 1978 .

[21]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Egocentrism as a Source of Unrealistic Optimism , 1982 .

[22]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Judged frequency of lethal events , 1978 .

[23]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .