Stretched and upside-down maps of auditory space in the optic tectum of blind-reared owls; acoustic basis and behavioral correlates

Vision during early life plays an important role in calibrating sound localization behavior. This study investigates the effects of visual deprivation on sound localization and on the neural representation of auditory space. Nine barn owls were raised with eyelids sutured closed; one owl was congenitally anophthalmic. Data from these birds were compared with data from owls raised with normal visual experience. Sound localization behavior was significantly less precise in blind- reared owls than in normal owls. The scatter of localization errors was particularly large in elevation, though it was abnormally large in both dimensions. However, there was no systematic bias to the localization errors measured over a range of source locations. This indicates that the representation of auditory space is degraded in some way for blind- reared owls, but on average is properly calibrated. The spatial tuning of auditory neurons in the optic tectum was studied in seven of the blind-reared owls to assess the effects of early visual deprivation on the neural representation of auditory space. In normal owls, units in the optic tectum are sharply tuned for sound source location and are organized systematically according to the locations of their receptive fields to form a map of auditory space. In blind-reared owls, the following auditory properties were abnormal: (1) auditory tuning for source elevation was abnormally broad, (2) the progression of the azimuths and elevations of auditory receptive fields across the tectum was erratic, and (3) in five of the seven owls, the auditory representation of elevation was systematically stretched, and in the two others large portions of the representation of elevation were flipped upside down. The following unit properties were apparently unaffected by blind rearing: (1) the sharpness of tuning for sound source azimuth, (2) the orientation of the auditory representation of azimuth, and (3) the mutual alignment of the auditory and visual receptive fields in the region of the tectum representing the area of space directly in front of the animal. The data demonstrate that the brain is capable of generating an auditory map of space without vision, but that the normal precision and topography of the map depend on visual experience. The space map results from the tuning of tectal units for interaural intensity differences (IIDs) and interaural time differences (ITDs; Olsen et al., 1989).

[1]  B. Gordon,et al.  Receptive fields in deep layers of cat superior colliculus. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  R. Hunt,et al.  Retinotectal specificity: models and experiments in search of a mapping function. , 1980, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  K. E. Binns,et al.  The Maturation of the Superior Collicular Map of Auditory Space in the Guinea Pig is Disrupted by Developmental Visual Deprivation , 1990, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  G. H. Fisher,et al.  SPATIAL LOCALIZATION BY THE BLIND. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[5]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Fused Binocular Vision is Required for Development of Proper Eye Alignment in Barn Owls , 1989, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Cowan,et al.  Specificity and plasticity of retinotectal connections: a computational model , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  D. M. Green,et al.  Directional sensitivity of sound-pressure levels in the human ear canal. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Neural maps of interaural time and intensity differences in the optic tectum of the barn owl , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  A. R. Palmer,et al.  A monaural space map in the guinea-pig superior colliculus , 1985, Hearing Research.

[10]  R Linsker,et al.  Perceptual neural organization: some approaches based on network models and information theory. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  E. Knudsen Experience alters the spatial tuning of auditory units in the optic tectum during a sensitive period in the barn owl , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  D. O'Leary,et al.  Inaccuracies in initial growth and arborization of chick retinotectal axons followed by course corrections and axon remodeling to develop topographic order , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Neural maps of head movement vector and speed in the optic tectum of the barn owl. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A J King,et al.  Spatial response properties of acoustically responsive neurons in the superior colliculus of the ferret: a map of auditory space. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  E. Knudsen,et al.  Sensitive and critical periods for visual calibration of sound localization by barn owls , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  C. Shatz,et al.  Interactions between retinal ganglion cells during the development of the mammalian visual system. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  D L Sparks,et al.  Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. , 1986, Physiological reviews.

[19]  B. Jones,et al.  Spatial perception in the blind. , 1975, British journal of psychology.

[20]  D. Irvine,et al.  Topographic organization of interaural intensity difference sensitivity in deep layers of cat superior colliculus: implications for auditory spatial representation. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  P F Knudsen,et al.  The sensitive period for auditory localization in barn owls is limited by age, not by experience , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Brian Lewis,et al.  Sound localization in birds , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  E. Knudsen Auditory and visual maps of space in the optic tectum of the owl , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  Andrew J. King,et al.  THE REPRESENTATION OF AUDITORY SPACE IN THE SUPERIOR COLLICULUS OF FERRETS RAISED WITH ABNORMAL BINAURAL CUES DURING POSTNATAL-DEVELOPMENT , 1986 .

[25]  S D Esterly,et al.  A critical period for the recovery of sound localization accuracy following monaural occlusion in the barn owl , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  A R Palmer,et al.  Cells responsive to free‐field auditory stimuli in guinea‐pig superior colliculus: distribution and response properties. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  M. P. Bryden,et al.  Effects of early and late blindness on auditory spatial learning in the rat , 1967 .

[28]  T C Yin,et al.  Responses of neurons in the cat's superior colliculus to acoustic stimuli. I. Monaural and binaural response properties. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  M. Konishi,et al.  A circuit for detection of interaural time differences in the brain stem of the barn owl , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  J. E. Hind,et al.  Interaural time differences: implications regarding the neurophysiology of sound localization. , 1980, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Trichur Raman Vidyasagar,et al.  Possible plasticity in the rat superior colliculus , 1978, Nature.

[32]  M. Konishi,et al.  Neural map of interaural phase difference in the owl's brainstem. , 1986, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  J M Harrison,et al.  Intensity changes at the ear as a function of the azimuth of a tone source: a comparative study. , 1970, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  D R Moore,et al.  A developmental study of the sound pressure transformation by the head of the cat. , 1979, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[35]  E. Knudsen Auditory properties of space-tuned units in owl's optic tectum. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  E. Knudsen Early auditory experience aligns the auditory map of space in the optic tectum of the barn owl. , 1983, Science.

[37]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  A neural code for auditory space in the cat's superior colliculus , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Developmental plasticity in the visual and auditory representations in the mammalian superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[39]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Early visual deprivation results in a degraded motor map in the optic tectum of barn owls. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  G. F. Kuhn Model for the interaural time differences in the azimuthal plane , 1977 .

[41]  H. Wagner,et al.  Representation of interaural time difference in the central nucleus of the barn owl's inferior colliculus , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.