Context change and retrieval difficulty in the list-before-last paradigm

Using the “list-before-last” paradigm (Jang & Huber, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 112–127, 2008; Shiffrin, Science, 168, 1601–1603, 1970), we examined whether the difficulty involved in retrieving a previously studied list affects the recall of the current list. Participants studied three lists (L1, L2, and L3), and between L2 and L3 study they either engaged in retrieval of L1 or solved math problems for the same duration of time. After L3 encoding, all participants recalled L2. We examined accurate recall of L2, intrusions from L3, and also the first-response function from L2 across all four experiments. In Experiments 1 and 2, retrieval difficulty was manipulated by presenting participants with variable numbers of letter cues for the L1 words, with some participants receiving more cues than others. In Experiment 1, L1 and L2 words shared the first two letters in common to create potential item interference, whereas in Experiment 2, they did not share common cues. In Experiments 3 and 4, we manipulated retrieval difficulty using a delay manipulation—L1 was encoded 1, 24, or 72 h prior to the session during which L2 and L3 were encoded. In Experiment 3, L1 retrieval involved a cued recall test, whereas in Experiment 4, it involved a free recall test. The results of all of these experiments showed that, as compared to solving math problems, retrieving L1 led to forgetting of L2, reduced intrusions from L3, and reduced first-response functions from L2. However, all of the dependent measures were invariant across the various manipulations of retrieval difficulty. We discuss the results in terms of the context-change interpretation of prior-list retrieval.

[1]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[2]  Lili Sahakyan,et al.  Remembering to Forget , 2010, Psychological science.

[3]  Benjamin C. Storm RETRIEVAL-INDUCED FORGETTING AND THE RESOLUTION OF COMPETITION , 2011 .

[4]  K. Malmberg,et al.  A global theory of remembering and forgetting from multiple lists. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  A. Benjamin Successful Remembering and Successful Forgetting : A Festschrift in Honor of Robert A. Bjork , 2011 .

[6]  R M Shiffrin,et al.  Forgetting: Trace Erosion or Retrieval Failure? , 1970, Science.

[7]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  On the puzzling relationship between environmental context and human memory. , 1989 .

[8]  W. Estes Statistical theory of spontaneous recovery and regression. , 1955, Psychological review.

[9]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Are the costs of directed forgetting due to failures of sampling or recovery? Exploring the dynamics of recall in list-method directed forgetting , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[10]  Jeffrey D. Karpicke,et al.  Test-Enhanced Learning , 2006, Psychological science.

[11]  J. Greenspoon,et al.  Stimulus conditions and retroactive inhibition. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  Steven M. Smith,et al.  Environmental context and human memory , 1978 .

[13]  Kathleen B McDermott,et al.  The testing effect in recognition memory: a dual process account. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Kent M. Dallett,et al.  Contextual stimuli and proactive inhibition , 1968 .

[15]  Colleen M. Kelley,et al.  A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  J. A. McGeoch Forgetting and the law of disuse. , 1932 .

[17]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Testing during study insulates against the buildup of proactive interference. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  Marc W. Howard,et al.  A distributed representation of temporal context , 2002 .

[19]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  On the relationship between interference and inhibition in cognition. , 2011 .

[20]  Kenneth J. Malmberg,et al.  The "one-shot" hypothesis for context storage. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  Lili Sahakyan Environmental context change affects memory for performed actions , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  C. Izawa Current Issues in Cognitive Processes : The Tulane Flowerree Symposia on Cognition , 2014 .

[23]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  K. Bäuml,et al.  Retrieval during learning facilitates subsequent memory encoding. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  S. Wakeman One Shot , 2013, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[26]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Search of associative memory. , 1981 .

[27]  H SCHLOSBERG,et al.  Similarity in stimulating conditions as a variable in retroactive inhibition. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. , 1975 .

[29]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  A model for interference and forgetting , 1988 .

[30]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. , 1988 .

[31]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  List-strength Effect: Ii. Theoretical Mechanisms , 1990 .

[32]  D. Huber,et al.  Context retrieval and context change in free recall: recalling from long-term memory drives list isolation. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Steven M. Smith,et al.  Environmental context-dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  John R. Anderson,et al.  RECOGNITION AND RETRIEVAL PROCESSES IN FREE RECALL , 1972 .