The Function and Organization of Lateral Prefrontal Cortex: A Test of Competing Hypotheses

The present experiment tested three hypotheses regarding the function and organization of lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC). The first account (the information cascade hypothesis) suggests that the anterior-posterior organization of lateral PFC is based on the timing with which cue stimuli reduce uncertainty in the action selection process. The second account (the levels-of-abstraction hypothesis) suggests that the anterior-posterior organization of lateral PFC is based on the degree of abstraction of the task goals. The current study began by investigating these two hypotheses, and identified several areas of lateral PFC that were predicted to be active by both the information cascade and levels-of-abstraction accounts. However, the pattern of activation across experimental conditions was inconsistent with both theoretical accounts. Specifically, an anterior area of mid-dorsolateral PFC exhibited sensitivity to experimental conditions that, according to both accounts, should have selectively engaged only posterior areas of PFC. We therefore investigated a third possible account (the adaptive context maintenance hypothesis) that postulates that both posterior and anterior regions of PFC are reliably engaged in task conditions requiring active maintenance of contextual information, with the temporal dynamics of activity in these regions flexibly tracking the duration of maintenance demands. Activity patterns in lateral PFC were consistent with this third hypothesis: regions across lateral PFC exhibited transient activation when contextual information had to be updated and maintained in a trial-by-trial manner, but sustained activation when contextual information had to be maintained over a series of trials. These findings prompt a reconceptualization of current views regarding the anterior-posterior organization of lateral PFC, but do support other findings regarding the active maintenance role of lateral PFC in sequential working memory paradigms.

[1]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[2]  J. Mugler,et al.  Three‐dimensional magnetization‐prepared rapid gradient‐echo imaging (3D MP RAGE) , 1990, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[3]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[4]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Rapid Automated Algorithm for Aligning and Reslicing PET Images , 1992, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[5]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Automated image registration , 1993 .

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Movement‐Related effects in fMRI time‐series , 1996, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[8]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Raichle,et al.  Anatomic Localization and Quantitative Analysis of Gradient Refocused Echo-Planar fMRI Susceptibility Artifacts , 1997, NeuroImage.

[10]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Automated image registration: I. General methods and intrasubject, intramodality validation. , 1998, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[12]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[13]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dopamine, cognitive control, and schizophrenia: the gating model. , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[14]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  James B. Rowe,et al.  Working Memory for Location and Time: Activity in Prefrontal Area 46 Relates to Selection Rather than Maintenance in Memory , 2001, NeuroImage.

[16]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and dynamic categorization tasks: representational organization and neuromodulatory control. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[18]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[19]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex dysfunction mediates deficits in working memory and prepotent responding in schizophrenia , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[20]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  Mixed blocked/event-related designs separate transient and sustained activity in fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[21]  C. Carter,et al.  Event-related FMRI study of context processing in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients with schizophrenia. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume , 2004, NeuroImage.

[23]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Specificity of prefrontal dysfunction and context processing deficits to schizophrenia in never-medicated patients with first-episode psychosis. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[24]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Prefrontal functioning during context processing in schizophrenia and major depression: An event-related fMRI study , 2005, Schizophrenia Research.

[25]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  M. Brass,et al.  Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta‐analyses of switching and Stroop studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[27]  E. Crone,et al.  Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Sang Joon Kim,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .

[30]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Making Working Memory Work: A Computational Model of Learning in the Prefrontal Cortex and Basal Ganglia , 2006, Neural Computation.

[31]  Katsuyuki Sakai,et al.  Prefrontal Set Activity Predicts Rule-Specific Neural Processing during Subsequent Cognitive Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Peter Fransson,et al.  Sustained and Transient Neural Modulations in Prefrontal Cortex Related to Declarative Long-Term Memory, Working Memory, and Attention , 2007, Cortex.

[33]  Peter Fransson,et al.  Unity and diversity of tonic and phasic executive control components in episodic and working memory , 2007, NeuroImage.

[34]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  T. Braver,et al.  Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .

[36]  Matthew M Botvinick,et al.  Multilevel structure in behaviour and in the brain: a model of Fuster's hierarchy , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[37]  David Badre,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence for a Hierarchical Organization of the Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  K. Sakai Task set and prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[39]  David Badre,et al.  Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  T. Braver,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: Differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  T. Braver,et al.  Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Temporal Dynamics of Cognitive Control , 2008, NIPS.

[43]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Developing PFC representations using reinforcement learning , 2009, Cognition.

[44]  Hannah S. Locke,et al.  Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Distinct neural circuits support transient and sustained processes in prospective memory and working memory. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  T. Braver,et al.  Attention, intention, and strategy in preparatory control , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  K. Christoff,et al.  Prefrontal organization of cognitive control according to levels of abstraction , 2009, Brain Research.

[48]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Agatha Lenartowicz,et al.  Updating of context in working memory: An event-related potential study , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[52]  T. Braver,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Interference Control in Working Memory: Effects of Interference Expectancy and Fluid Intelligence , 2010, PloS one.

[53]  B. Gold,et al.  Common and Distinct Mechanisms of Cognitive Flexibility in Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.