Does the Relation Between the Control of Attention and Second Language Proficiency Generalize From India to Canada?

Over the last decades, the extralinguistic benefits of bilingualism have been intensively debated. The current study was aimed at clarifying whether bilingualism speeds attentional disengagement. Reflecting faster disengagement, Mishra, Hilchey, Singh, and Klein (2012) observed an earlier onset of inhibition of return (IOR) for high than for low-proficient bilinguals. In contrast, Hernandez, Costa, Fuentes, Vivas, and Sebastian-Galles (2010) failed to find any difference between bilinguals and monolinguals. We investigated the source of this discrepancy, while improving methodology by using a large sample composed of 100 Canadians, objective assessments of second language skills (Nelson-Denny Reading test), and controlling for nonverbal intelligence, age, sex, and video-gaming. Results were analyzed with self-report and objective measures of second language proficiency as well as dichotomous and continuous measures. Compared to less proficient bilinguals, highly proficient bilinguals tended to respond faster overall, hinting at an executive processing advantage. However, contrary to Mishra et al.’s findings, bilingual proficiency did not affect either the onset of IOR or magnitude of IOR. Résumé Au cours des dernières décennies, les avantages extralinguistiques du bilinguisme ont fait l’objet de vifs débats. L’étude actuelle visait à déterminer si le bilinguisme accélérait le désengagement attentionnel. Reflétant un désengagement plus rapide, Mishra, Hilchey, Singh, et Klein (2012) ont observé une manifestation plus hâtive de l’inhibition de retour (IOR) chez les bilingues moins compétents dans leur langue seconde. En revanche, Hernandez, Costa, Fuentes, Vivas et Sebastian-Galles (2010) n’ont pas réussi à trouver de différence entre les bilingues et les monolingues. Nous avons étudié l’origine de cette différence tout en améliorant la méthodologie au moyen d’un large échantillon composé de 100 Canadiens, d’évaluations objectives des compétences en langue seconde (test de lecture Nelson-Denny) et en contrôlant différents paramètres tels que l’intelligence non-verbale, l’âge, le sexe et l’utilisation de jeux vidéo. Les résultats ont été analysés au moyen d’auto-déclarations et de mesures objectives des compétences dans la langue seconde et des mesures en continu et dichotomiques. Par comparaison avec les bilingues moins compétents dans leur langue seconde, leurs semblables plus compétents avaient tendance à répondre plus rapidement dans l’ensemble, donnant à penser qu’ils avaient un avantage sur le plan du traitement exécutif. Or, contrairement aux constats de Mishra et al., le niveau de compétence dans la langue seconde n’avait pas de conséquences sur l’apparition de l’IOR ni sur l’ampleur de l’IOR.

[1]  Deanna C. Friesen,et al.  Sequential congruency effects reveal differences in disengagement of attention for monolingual and bilingual young adults , 2017, Cognition.

[2]  E. Bialystok The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. , 2017, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  M. Posner,et al.  Integrating brain, cognition and culture , 2017 .

[4]  R. Klein What cognitive processes are likely to be exercised by bilingualism and does this exercise lead to extra-linguistic cognitive benefits? , 2016 .

[5]  Alessandra S Souza,et al.  No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  R. Klein On the belief that the cognitive exercise associated with the acquisition of a second language enhances extra-linguistic cognitive functions: Is “Type-I incompetence” at work here? , 2015, Cortex.

[7]  K. Paap,et al.  Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances , 2015, Cortex.

[8]  E. Bialystok Bilingualism and the Development of Executive Function: The Role of Attention. , 2015, Child development perspectives.

[9]  S. Della Sala,et al.  Cognitive Advantage in Bilingualism , 2015, Psychological science.

[10]  Virginia Valian Bilingualism and cognition , 2014, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[11]  Andrea Stocco,et al.  Bilingualism trains specific brain circuits involved in flexible rule selection and application , 2014, Brain and Language.

[12]  D. Titone,et al.  Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging , 2014, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[13]  K. Paap,et al.  Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? , 2014 .

[14]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.

[15]  K. Paap,et al.  There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  F. Craik,et al.  Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Matthew D. Hilchey,et al.  On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: Higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  D. Salmon,et al.  Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Matthew D. Hilchey,et al.  Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  Albert Costa,et al.  The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention , 2010 .

[21]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  J. Morton,et al.  What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. , 2007, Developmental science.

[24]  Julie I. Cohen,et al.  Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  R. Klein,et al.  The effects of memory load on the time course of inhibition of return , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  F. Craik,et al.  Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[27]  A. Samuel,et al.  Inhibition of return: A graphical meta-analysis of its time course and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  Janice J. Snyder,et al.  Attentional momentum does not underlie the inhibition of return effect , 2001 .

[29]  E. Bialystok Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. , 2001 .

[30]  Raymond Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  E. Bialystok Cognitive Complexity and Attentional Control in the Bilingual Mind , 1999 .

[32]  D. Green,et al.  Lemma selection without inhibition of languages in bilingual speakers , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[33]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .

[34]  R. Klein,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingual Processing: Does bilingual exercise enhance cognitive fitness in traditional non-linguistic executive processing tasks? , 2015 .

[35]  R. Klein On the Role of Endogenous Orienting in the Inhibitory Aftermath of Exogenous Orienting , 2005 .

[36]  J. Pratt,et al.  Examining the effect of practice on inhibition of return in static displays , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  Elizabeth Peal,et al.  The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. , 1962 .

[38]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[39]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews the Spatial Orienting Paradigm: How to Design and Interpret Spatial Attention Experiments , 2022 .