Visual feedback attenuates force fluctuations induced by a stressor.

PURPOSE The fluctuations in force during a steady contraction can be influenced by age, vision, and level of physiological arousal. The aim of this study was to determine the effects of a stressor on the force fluctuations and information transmission exhibited by young, middle-aged, and older adults when a pinch-grip task was performed with and without visual feedback. METHODS Thirty-six men and women (19-86 yr) participated in a protocol that comprised anticipatory (30 min), stressor (15 min), and recovery periods (25 min). The stressor was a series of noxious electrical stimuli applied to the dorsal surface of the left hand. Subjects sustained a pinch-grip force with the right hand at 2% of the maximal voluntary contraction force. The normalized fluctuations in pinch-grip force (coefficient of variation), information transmission (log2 signal:noise), and the spectra for the force were quantified across the 70-min protocol. RESULTS Removal of visual feedback exacerbated the force fluctuations (3.83+/- 3.15 vs 2.82+/- 1.64%) and reduced the information transmission (5.01+/- 0.86 vs 5.34+/- 0.71 bits) only during the stressor period. The effect was similar for all age groups. Older adults exhibited greater force fluctuations and lower information transmission compared with young and middle-aged adults, especially during the stressor period. The impairments in fine motor performance during the stressor were associated with an enhancement of the power at 1-2 Hz in the force spectrum (R=0.41-0.52). CONCLUSION Removal of visual feedback increased the force fluctuations and decreased information transmission during a stressor period, which suggests that integration of visual feedback can attenuate the stressor-induced enhancement of synaptic input received by the motor neuron pool.

[1]  Christopher A Knight,et al.  Enhanced motor unit rate coding with improvements in a force-matching task. , 2004, Journal of electromyography and kinesiology : official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology.

[2]  R. Enoka,et al.  The 1- to 2-Hz oscillations in muscle force are exacerbated by stress, especially in older adults. , 2004, Journal of applied physiology.

[3]  Roger M Enoka,et al.  Multiple features of motor-unit activity influence force fluctuations during isometric contractions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  C. Heckman Active Conductances in Motoneuron Dendrites Enhance Movement Capabilities , 2003, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[5]  Karl M Newell,et al.  Aging and the time and frequency structure of force output variability. , 2003, Journal of applied physiology.

[6]  R. Enoka,et al.  Mechanisms that contribute to differences in motor performance between young and old adults. , 2003, Journal of electromyography and kinesiology : official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology.

[7]  Martin Falcke,et al.  Buffers and oscillations in intracellular Ca2+ dynamics. , 2003, Biophysical journal.

[8]  K. Newell,et al.  Time-dependent structure in the discharge rate of human motor units , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  H. Johansson,et al.  Sympathetic modulation of muscle spindle afferent sensitivity to stretch in rabbit jaw closing muscles , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  L G Carlton,et al.  Old adults exhibit greater motor output variability than young adults only during rapid discrete isometric contractions. , 2001, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[11]  R. Enoka,et al.  Activation of the arousal response and impairment of performance increase with anxiety and stressor intensity. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[12]  R. Enoka,et al.  Activation of the arousal response can impair performance on a simple motor task. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[13]  Ramón Huerta,et al.  Regularization mechanisms of spiking-bursting neurons , 2001, Neural Networks.

[14]  C. E. SHANNON,et al.  A mathematical theory of communication , 1948, MOCO.

[15]  K. Newell,et al.  Intermittency in the control of continuous force production. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Allen I. Selverston,et al.  Modeling observed chaotic oscillations in bursting neurons: the role of calcium dynamics and IP3 , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[17]  A. Selverston,et al.  Interacting biological and electronic neurons generate realistic oscillatory rhythms , 2000, Neuroreport.

[18]  E. Ribot-Ciscar,et al.  Increased muscle spindle sensitivity to movement during reinforcement manoeuvres in relaxed human subjects , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  P. Brown Cortical drives to human muscle: the Piper and related rhythms , 2000, Progress in Neurobiology.

[20]  C. Christakos,et al.  Occurrence of widespread motor-unit firing correlations in muscle contractions: their role in the generation of tremor and time-varying voluntary force. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Persistence in visual feedback control by the elderly , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  C. Bard,et al.  Age-related differences in the integration of sensory information during the execution of a bimanual coordination task. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  K. Kanda,et al.  Dynamic and static sensitivities of muscle spindle primary endings in aged rats to ramp stretch , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[24]  C. J. Luca,et al.  Common drive of motor units in regulation of muscle force , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  C. D. De Luca,et al.  Control scheme governing concurrently active human motor units during voluntary contractions , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  V. Dietz,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms underlying physiological tremor. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communications , 1948 .