The class inclusion question: a case study in applying pragmatics to the experimental study of cognition

For more than 70 years, Piaget’s class-inclusion task (given, e.g., five asters and three tulips, the child is asked whether “there are more asters or more flowers”) has been the object of experimental investigation. Inclusion is of considerable importance for cognitive science as it is a key concept for logical operations and knowledge representation. It is shown that the question can be characterised by a kind of privative ambiguity which is at the source of the younger children’s answer, “more asters”. A relevance-theoretic explanation of children’s interpretation of the question and of the subsequent responses is expounded. This account can explain the effect of all the factors that are known to influence performance (e.g., role of collections, counting, typicality, qualification, syntax, etc.), a review of which is presented. It is further tested experimentally. The development of performance is explained on the basis of the way children disambiguate the question. This study exemplifies the two ways in which pragmatic analysis is pertinent to the study of children’s (as well as adults’) reasoning and judgement, namely in explaining and predicting participants’ comprehension of the statements and questions, and in taking into account attribution processes that occur in the experimental setting.

[1]  Gregory L Murphy,et al.  An apple is more than just a fruit: cross-classification in children's concepts. , 2003, Child Development.

[2]  V. Reyna,et al.  Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis , 1995 .

[3]  G. Winer,et al.  Class-Inclusion Reasoning in Children: A Review of the Empirical Literature. , 1980 .

[4]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  The informative functions of research procedures: Bias and the logic of conversation. , 1993 .

[5]  L. Siegel,et al.  Evidence for the Understanding of Class Inclusion in Preschool Children: Linguistic Factors and Training Effects. , 1978 .

[6]  C. Brainerd Training and Transfer of Transitivity, Conservation, and Class Inclusion of Length. , 1974 .

[7]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Telling what they want to know: participants tailor causal attributions to researchers' interests , 1999 .

[8]  C. Brainerd,et al.  AN ANALYSIS OF TWO PROPOSED SOURCES OF CHILDREN'S CLASS INCLUSION ERRORS , 1974 .

[9]  Frances Horibe,et al.  Development of the Appreciation of Verbal Jokes. , 1974 .

[10]  E. Markman,et al.  Classes and collections: Internal organization and resulting holistic properties , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  A. Garton,et al.  On the young child's comparison of sets. , 1981, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  F. Agnoli Development of judgmental heuristics and logical reasoning: Training counteracts the representativeness heuristic , 1991 .

[13]  J. Piaget,et al.  The early growth of logic in the child : classification and seriation , 1965 .

[14]  M. K. Lane,et al.  Role of atypical exemplars of social and nonsocial superordinate categories within the class inclusion paradigm. , 1985 .

[15]  B. Hodkin Performance Model Analysis in Class Inclusion: An Illustration with Two Language Conditions. , 1987 .

[16]  La psychologie du raisonnement : lois de la pragmatique et logique formelle , 1993 .

[17]  Classes, collections, and distinctive features: Alternative strategies for solving inclusion problems , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  R. Lerner,et al.  Class-inclusion failure: cognitive deficit or misleading reference? , 1974, Child development.

[19]  S. Waxman,et al.  Beyond the basics: preschool children label objects flexibly at multiple hierarchical levels , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[20]  E. Markman The Facilitation of Part-Whole Comparisons by Use of the Collective Noun "Family". , 1973 .

[21]  Carolyn B. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of subordinate categories by 3-year-olds: The roles of attribute salience, linguistic input, and child characteristics , 1994 .

[22]  R. Grieve,et al.  Interpreting inclusion: A contribution to the study of the child's cognitive and linguistic development , 1978 .

[23]  Responses to class-inclusion questions for verbally and pictorially presented items. , 1968, Child development.

[24]  Lynn S. Liben,et al.  Piaget and the foundations of knowledge , 1983 .

[25]  D. Hilton THE SOCIAL CONTEXT OF REASONING : CONVERSATIONAL INFERENCE AND RATIONAL JUDGMENT , 1995 .

[26]  Scott A Miller,et al.  Children's understanding of the necessity of logically necessary truths , 2000 .

[27]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Classes and collections: Conceptual organization and numerical abilities , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  Bärbel Inhelder,et al.  Learning and the Development of Cognition , 1974 .

[29]  Marilyn Shatz,et al.  Two-year-olds' recognition of hierarchies , 2001 .

[30]  E. Shipley The class-inclusion task: Question form and distributive comparisons , 1979, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[31]  C. Mervis,et al.  Learning to Solve Class-Inclusion Problems: The Roles of Quantification and Recognition of Contradiction. , 1979 .

[32]  G. Kohnstamm An evaluation of part of Piaget's theory , 1963 .

[33]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Cognition and Communication: Judgmental Biases, Research Methods, and the Logic of Conversation , 2014 .

[34]  J. Sadock Speech acts , 2007 .

[35]  Willis F. Overton,et al.  Reasoning, necessity, and logic: Developmental perspectives. , 2013 .

[36]  Class inclusion and conclusions about Piaget's theory , 1982 .

[37]  Deena K. Bernstein,et al.  The development of humor: implications for assessment and intervention , 1986 .

[38]  J. L. Sheppard Conservation of Part and Whole in the Acquisition of Class Inclusion. , 1973 .

[39]  B. Hodkin Language Effects in Assessment of Class-inclusion Ability. , 1981 .

[40]  P. Blewitt,et al.  Understanding Categorical Hierarchies: The Earliest Levels of Skill , 1994 .

[41]  James W. Hall,et al.  Effects of collection terms on class-inclusion and on number tasks , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  G. Winer An Analysis of Verbal Facilitation of Class-Inclusion Reasoning. , 1974 .

[43]  J. Youniss,et al.  Reasons for Failure on the Class Inclusion Problem. , 1970 .

[44]  The Construction of Logical Necessity , 1982 .

[45]  I. Spence,et al.  Class-Inclusion Reasoning: Patterns of Performance from Three to Eight Years , 1982 .

[46]  Margaret C. Donaldson,et al.  Conservation accidents , 1975, Cognition.

[47]  H. Thomas,et al.  Competency criteria and the class inclusion task: modeling judgments and justifications. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[48]  B. Sutton‐Smith A Developmental Structural Account of Riddles. , 1973 .

[49]  T. Shultz Development of the Appreciation of Riddles. , 1974 .

[50]  Children's Understanding of the Referential Properties of Collective and Class Nouns. , 1982 .

[51]  Maureen A. Callanan,et al.  Development of Object Categories and Inclusion Relations: Preschoolers' Hypotheses about Word Meanings. , 1989 .

[52]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[53]  V. Reyna,et al.  Inclusion illusions: Fuzzy-trace theory and perceptual salience effects in cognitive development , 1990 .

[54]  V. Reyna Class inclusion, the conjunction fallacy, and other cognitive illusions , 1991 .

[55]  J. Smedslund CONCRETE REASONING: A STUDY OF INTELLECTUAL DEVELOPMENT. , 1964, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[56]  C. Brainerd,et al.  On the independence of short-term memory and working memory in cognitive development , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[57]  J. Hayes THE CHILD'S CONCEPTION OF THE EXPERIMENTER , 1972 .

[58]  Some Members Are More Equal than Others: The Effect of Semantic Typicality on class-Inclusion performance , 1976 .

[59]  Carpendale,et al.  Language and Operations in Children's Class Inclusion Reasoning: The Operational Semantic Theory of Reasoning , 1996, Developmental review : DR.

[60]  P. Cormier,et al.  Class-Inclusion Developmental Levels and Logical Necessity , 1983 .

[61]  G. Winer,et al.  Children'S Responses to Verbally and Pictorially Presented Class-Inclusion Items and to a Task of Number Conservation , 1974 .

[62]  C. J. Brainerd,et al.  Autosuggestibility in Memory Development , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[63]  A. Padilla,et al.  Verbal Facilitation of Class-Inclusion Reasoning: Children Tested in Their Dominant or Subordinate Language , 1976 .

[64]  S. Gelman,et al.  Incorporating new words into the lexicon: preliminary evidence for language hierarchies in two-year-old children. , 1989, Child development.

[65]  Guy Politzer,et al.  Reasoning, Judgement and Pragmatics , 2004 .

[66]  Development of children's understanding of basic-subordinate inclusion relations. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[67]  Elizabeth F. Shipley,et al.  A constraint on comparisons: Equally detailed alternatives , 1983 .

[68]  J. H. Tatarsky The influence of dimensional manipulations on class-inclusion performance. , 1974, Child development.

[69]  J. Wohlwill RESPONSES TO CLASS-INCLUSION QUESTIONS FOR VERBALLY AND PICTORIALLY PRESENTED ITEMS , 1968 .

[70]  The effect of class and collection labels on cardinality, class-inclusion, and number conservation tasks. , 1988, Child development.

[71]  The Effects of Linguistic Factors on Class Inclusion Performance in Adults and Children , 1984 .

[72]  Ines Gloeckner,et al.  Relevance Communication And Cognition , 2016 .

[73]  G. Winer Enhancement of Class-Inclusion Reasoning Through Verbal Context. , 1978 .

[74]  A. Karmiloff-Smith From meta-processes to conscious access: Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data , 1986, Cognition.

[75]  Failure in Class-Inclusion Reasoning in a University Sample , 1987 .

[76]  A. Wilkinson Counting strategies and semantic analysis as applied to class inclusion , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[77]  David Moshman,et al.  The development of metalogical understanding , 1996 .

[78]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1997 .