Developmental patterns of affective attention across the first 2 years of life

Abstract This study examined patterns of attention toward affective stimuli in a longitudinal sample of typically developing infants (N = 357, 147 females, 50% White, 22% Latinx, 16% African American/Black, 3% Asian, 8% mixed race, 1% not reported) using two eye‐tracking tasks that measure vigilance to (rapid detection), engagement with (total looking toward), and disengagement from (latency to looking away) emotional facial configurations. Infants completed each task at 4, 8, 12, 18, and 24 months of age from 2016 to 2020. Multilevel growth models demonstrate that, over the first 2 years of life, infants became faster at detecting and spent more time engaging with angry over neutral faces. These results have implications for our understanding of the development of affect‐biased attention.

[1]  Jessica L. Burris,et al.  Moderating effects of environmental stressors on the development of attention to threat in infancy. , 2022, Developmental psychobiology.

[2]  S. Morales,et al.  Variable- and person-centered approaches to affect-biased attention in infancy reveal unique relations with infant negative affect and maternal anxiety , 2021, Scientific Reports.

[3]  S. Morales,et al.  Temperament moderates developmental changes in vigilance to emotional faces in infants: Evidence from an eye-tracking study. , 2020, Developmental psychobiology.

[4]  B. Repacholi,et al.  The Development of Negative Event-Emotion Matching in Infancy: Implications for Theories in Affective Science , 2020, Affective Science.

[5]  Jessica L. Burris,et al.  Biased attention to threat and anxiety: On taking a developmental approach , 2019, Journal of Experimental Psychopathology.

[6]  Atsuko Nakagawa,et al.  Attentional bias assessed by a facial expression cuing paradigm in infants , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[7]  J. Leppänen,et al.  Infants' attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. , 2018, Developmental science.

[8]  C. Nelson,et al.  Early development of attention to threat-related facial expressions , 2018, PloS one.

[9]  R. Wiers,et al.  Association between attention bias to threat and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents , 2018, Depression and anxiety.

[10]  Jessica L. Burris,et al.  An Eye Tracking Investigation of Attentional Biases Towards Affect in Young Children , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[11]  S. Morales,et al.  Maternal Anxiety Predicts Attentional Bias Towards Threat in Infancy , 2017, Emotion.

[12]  S. Morales,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience a Developmental Neuroscience Perspective on Affect-biased Attention , 2022 .

[13]  B. Hordacre,et al.  Minimum number of trials required for within- and between-session reliability of TMS measures of corticospinal excitability , 2016, Neuroscience.

[14]  Dima Amso,et al.  The attentive brain: insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[16]  R. Meesen,et al.  Optimization of the Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Protocol by Defining a Reliable Estimate for Corticospinal Excitability , 2014, PloS one.

[17]  Jari K. Hietanen,et al.  The Emergence and Stability of the Attentional Bias to Fearful Faces in Infancy , 2013 .

[18]  Thomas Armstrong,et al.  Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. , 2012, Clinical psychology review.

[19]  Atsuko Nakagawa,et al.  Difficulty in disengaging from threat and temperamental negative affectivity in early life: A longitudinal study of infants aged 12–36 months , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[20]  Adam K. Anderson,et al.  Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  Brad E. Sheese,et al.  Developing Mechanisms of Self-Regulation in Early Life , 2011, Emotion review : journal of the International Society for Research on Emotion.

[23]  A. Field,et al.  Is There Room for ‘Development’ in Developmental Models of Information Processing Biases to Threat in Children and Adolescents? , 2010, Clinical child and family psychology review.

[24]  Christine L. Larson,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Visual Cognition What Makes an Angry Face Look so … Angry? Examining Visual Attention to the Shape of Threat in Children and Adults , 2022 .

[25]  E. Koster,et al.  Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. , 2010, Clinical psychology review.

[26]  J. Tanaka,et al.  The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[27]  J. Hietanen,et al.  Fearful faces but not fearful eyes alone delay attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants. , 2009, Emotion.

[28]  J. Hietanen,et al.  Emergence of enhanced attention to fearful faces between 5 and 7 months of age. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[29]  O. Pascalis,et al.  The Origins of Face Processing in Humans: Phylogeny and Ontogeny , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[30]  Vanessa Lobue,et al.  More than just another face in the crowd: superior detection of threatening facial expressions in children and adults. , 2009, Developmental science.

[31]  Karen E Adolph,et al.  When infants take mothers' advice: 18-month-olds integrate perceptual and social information to guide motor action. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  T. Striano,et al.  Developmental changes in infants’ processing of happy and angry facial expressions: A neurobehavioral study , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[34]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The perception of facial expressions in newborns , 2007, The European journal of developmental psychology.

[35]  R. Kleck,et al.  Perceived Gaze Direction and the Processing of Facial Displays of Emotion , 2003, Psychological science.

[36]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  The infant as onlooker: learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. , 2003, Child development.

[37]  Andrew W Young,et al.  The eyebrow frown: a salient social signal. , 2002, Emotion.

[38]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  K. Willmes,et al.  On the Functional Neuroanatomy of Intrinsic and Phasic Alertness , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Infants' fast saccades in the gap paradigm and development of visual attention☆ , 1997 .

[41]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  Infants' responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. , 1996, Child development.

[42]  C. Umilta,et al.  Face preference at birth. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Disengaging visual attention in the infant and adult , 1993 .

[44]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline , 1991, Cognition.

[45]  Marshall M. Haith,et al.  Young Infants' Visual Expectations for Symmetric and Asymmetric Stimulus Sequences. , 1991 .

[46]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[47]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  The perception of facial expressions by the three-month-old. , 1981, Child development.

[48]  C. Izard,et al.  Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. , 1976, Child development.

[49]  F. L. Clayton,et al.  Infant visual fixation on motion pictures of the human face. , 1968, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[50]  S. Morales,et al.  Study protocol: longitudinal attention and temperament study (LAnTs) , 2021 .

[51]  J. Fagard,et al.  Development and learning of saccadic eye movements in 7- to 42-month-old children. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[52]  Sabrina Eberhart,et al.  Applied Missing Data Analysis , 2016 .

[53]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[54]  J. Deloache,et al.  Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. , 2010, Developmental science.

[55]  J. Hietanen,et al.  Fearful faces modulate looking duration and attention disengagement in 7-month-old infants. , 2008, Developmental science.

[56]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  J. Colombo The development of visual attention in infancy. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[58]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[59]  J. Atkinson,et al.  EARLY DEVELOPMENT OF THE CONTROL OF VISUAL-ATTENTION , 1985 .

[60]  J. Campos,et al.  Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. , 1985 .