Recall of actions, sentences, and nouns: Influences of adult age and passage of time☆

Abstract The drop in performance between immediate free recall (IFR) and final free recall (FFR) was compared for action events (subject-performed tasks or SPTs), and two types of verbal materials (sentences and nouns). The results showed a more pronounced drop in performance from IFR to FFR for the verbal materials than for the SPTs. This finding was discussed in terms of SPTs being less sensitive to proactive interference than verbal materials, resulting in less forgetting over the initial minutes of retention for SPTs. In addition, age differences in recall of the same types of materials were examined for one group of young subjects ( M = 19.1 years) and one group of old subjects ( M = 69.3 years). Differences in recall, favouring the young, were more pronounced for the verbal materials than for the SPTs.

[1]  F. Craik,et al.  On the Transfer of Information from Temporary to Permanent Memory [and Discussion] , 1983 .

[2]  M. Dick,et al.  Memory for action events in Alzheimer-type dementia: Further evidence of an encoding failure , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  F. Craik,et al.  Depth of processing and the retention of words , 1975 .

[4]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Evaluating forgetting curves. , 1985 .

[5]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  Aging and memory for words and action events: effects of item repetition and list length. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[6]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  On the generality of some memory laws , 1981 .

[7]  F. Craik,et al.  Additive and interactive effects in memory for subject-performed tasks , 1990 .

[8]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Implicit memory and the enactment of verbal instructions , 1989 .

[9]  N. J. Slamecka,et al.  The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon , 1978 .

[10]  W. Lichty,et al.  Adult age differences in memory for motor versus cognitive activities. , 1986, Experimental aging research.

[11]  M. Knopf,et al.  Aging and Memory for Action Events: The Role of Familiarity. , 1989 .

[12]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Encoding dimensions of subject-performed tasks , 1991 .

[13]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Prerequisites for lack of age differences in memory performance. , 1985, Experimental aging research.

[14]  Brian McElree,et al.  Normal forgetting of verbal lists as a function of their degree of learning. , 1983 .

[15]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Recall of enacted and nonenacted instructions compared: forgetting functions , 1989 .

[16]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Aging effects in free recall: An exception to the rule. , 1984 .

[17]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  The effect of encoding variables on the free recall of words and action events , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[18]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  How to avoid developmental effects in free recall , 1982 .

[19]  R. L. Cohen Memory for action events: The power of enactment , 1989 .

[20]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  Memory in educable mentally retarded adults: deficit in subject or experimenter? , 1983 .

[21]  Donald H. Kausler,et al.  Experimental Psychology and Human Aging , 1982 .

[22]  R. R. Hunt,et al.  Adult age differences in memory for imagined and performed actions. , 1988, Journal of gerontology.

[23]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Motor programs and their relation to semantic memory. , 1985 .

[24]  D. Rubin,et al.  The effects of aging on perceived and generated memories , 1989 .

[25]  N. J. Slamecka,et al.  On comparing rates of forgetting: Comment on Loftus (1985). , 1985 .

[26]  L. Backman Further evidence for the lack of adult age differences on free recall of subject-performed tasks: The importance of motor action. , 1985 .

[27]  L. Nyberg,et al.  A component analysis of action events , 1991 .