Innovation does not indicate behavioral flexibility in great-tailed grackles

Many cross-species studies attest that innovation frequency (novel food types eaten and foraging techniques used) is a measure of behavioral flexibility and show that it positively correlates with relative brain size (corrected for body size). However, mixed results from the three studies that directly test the relationship between innovation frequency and behavioral flexibility and behavioral flexibility and brain size question both assumptions. I investigated behavioral flexibility in non-innovative great-tailed grackles that have an average sized brain, and compared their test performance with innovative, large-brained New Caledonian crows. Contrary to the prediction, grackles perform similarly to crows in experiments using clear tubes partially filled with water and containing a floating food reward, where objects must be dropped into the tube to raise the water level, bringing the food within reach. Similarly to crows, 4 out of 6 grackles preferred to drop the more functional heavy (rather than light) objects, and 2 changed their preference in a follow up experiment where the heavy objects were no longer functional, thus exhibiting behavioral flexibility. These results challenge the assumption that innovation frequency indicates behavioral flexibility since a non-innovative bird demonstrated behavioral flexibility at a level similar to that in innovative crows, and they challenge the assumption that only large brains are capable of behavioral flexibility because a bird with an average brain size solved problems similarly to large-brained crows.

[1]  D. H. Olster,et al.  Evolution of Bower Complexity and Cerebellum Size in Bowerbirds , 2005, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[2]  J. Nelson,et al.  A Comparative Analysis of Relative Brain Size in Waterfowl (Anseriformes) , 2001, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[3]  J. Call,et al.  Repeated innovation in great apes , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Neural components underlying behavioral flexibility in human reversal learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  FOREBRAIN SIZE AND INNOVATION RATE IN EUROPEAN BIRDS: FEEDING, NESTING AND CONFOUNDING VARIABLES , 2000 .

[6]  J. Erritzøe,et al.  Covariation between brain size and immunity in birds: implications for brain size evolution , 2005, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[7]  J. Nelson,et al.  A comparative test of the correlated evolution of flightlessness and relative brain size in birds , 2004 .

[8]  Christopher D. Bird,et al.  Rooks Use Stones to Raise the Water Level to Reach a Floating Worm , 2009, Current Biology.

[9]  Russell D. Gray,et al.  Species-wide manufacture of stick-type tools by New Caledonian Crows , 2002 .

[10]  H. Frahm,et al.  Allometric Comparison of the Brain and Brain Structures in the White Crested Polish Chicken with Uncrested Domestic Chicken Breeds , 1998, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[11]  Grass-Stem Tool use in New Caledonian Crows Corvus moneduloides , 2008 .

[12]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[13]  H. Frahm,et al.  Mosaic Evolution and Adaptive Brain Component Alteration under Domestication Seen on the Background of Evolutionary Theory , 2007, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[14]  H. Akaike Likelihood of a model and information criteria , 1981 .

[15]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Model selection and multimodel inference : a practical information-theoretic approach , 2003 .

[16]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  New Caledonian Crows Learn the Functional Properties of Novel Tool Types , 2011, PloS one.

[17]  Cameron Buckner A property cluster theory of cognition , 2015 .

[18]  Andrew N. Iwaniuk,et al.  Interspecific Allometry of the Brain and Brain Regions in Parrots (Psittaciformes): Comparisons with Other Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[19]  A. S. Griffin,et al.  Innovative problem solving in nonhuman animals: the effects of group size revisited , 2015 .

[20]  I. Teschke,et al.  The tale of the finch: adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  K. Zilles,et al.  Quantitative Development of Brain and Brain Structures in Birds (Galliformes and Passeriformes) Compared to that in Mammals (Insectivores and Primates) (Part 2 of 2) , 1991 .

[22]  Candy Rowe,et al.  A critique of comparative studies of brain size , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Brains, Innovations and Evolution in Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[24]  H. Frahm,et al.  Discontinuous Variability of Brain Composition among Domestic Chicken Breeds , 2003, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[25]  Gavin R. Hunt,et al.  Tool Use by the New Caledonian Crow Corvus moneduloides to Obtain Cerambycidae from Dead Wood , 2000 .

[26]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Ecological generalism and behavioural innovation in birds: technical intelligence or the simple incorporation of new foods? , 2015, The Journal of animal ecology.

[28]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Video Cameras on Wild Birds , 2007, Science.

[29]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius) , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[30]  Introduced Lantana camara used as tools by New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) , 2008 .

[31]  Shinichi Nakagawa,et al.  General Quantitative Genetic Methods for Comparative Biology , 2014 .

[32]  Russell D. Gray,et al.  Direct observations of pandanus-tool manufacture and use by a New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides) , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[33]  K. Zilles,et al.  Quantitative development of brain and brain structures in birds (galliformes and passeriformes) compared to that in mammals (insectivores and primates). , 1991, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[34]  Russell D. Gray,et al.  Modifications to the Aesop's Fable Paradigm Change New Caledonian Crow Performances , 2014, PloS one.

[35]  Alex H. Taylor,et al.  Using the Aesop's Fable Paradigm to Investigate Causal Understanding of Water Displacement by New Caledonian Crows , 2014, PloS one.

[36]  M. Gustafson Identification Guide to North American Birds. Part II , 2009 .

[37]  D. Boire,et al.  Allometric comparison of brain and main brain subdivisions in birds. , 1994, Journal fur Hirnforschung.

[38]  P. Ebinger Domestication and plasticity of brain organization in mallards (Anas platyrhynchos). , 1995, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[39]  J. Nelson,et al.  Developmental differences are correlated with relative brain size in birds: a comparative analysis , 2003 .

[40]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Relative Size of the Hyperstriatum ventrale Is the Best Predictor of Feeding Innovation Rate in Birds , 2000, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[41]  Alan C Kamil,et al.  Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). , 2007, Journal of comparative psychology.

[42]  P. Hurd,et al.  The Evolution of Cerebrotypes in Birds , 2005, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[43]  H. J. Jerison Animal intelligence as encephalization. , 1985, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[44]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Innovating Innovation Rate and Its Relationship with Brains, Ecology and General Intelligence , 2013, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[45]  C. Tristram,et al.  Notes on the Avifauna of New Caledonia , 2008 .

[46]  N. Clayton,et al.  How Do Children Solve Aesop's Fable? , 2012, PloS one.

[47]  Jarrod Hadeld MCMCglmm Course Notes , 2012 .

[48]  G. Hunt Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.

[49]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Innovative foraging behaviour in birds: What characterizes an innovator? , 2011, Behavioural Processes.

[50]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm , 2011, PloS one.

[51]  Andrew N. Iwaniuk,et al.  The evolution of stereopsis and the Wulst in caprimulgiform birds: a comparative analysis , 2006, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[52]  G. Crile,et al.  A Record of the Body Weight and Certain Organ and Gland Weights of 3690 Animals , 1940 .

[53]  E. Pistone,et al.  Tamaño relativo encefálico e índices cerebrales en Vanellus c. chilensis (Aves: Charadriidae) , 2002 .

[54]  J. Hadfield,et al.  General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi‐trait models for continuous and categorical characters , 2010, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[55]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[56]  A. Iwaniuk Brood Parasitism and Brain Size in Cuckoos: A Cautionary Tale on the Use of Modern Comparative Methods , 2004 .

[57]  K. Laland,et al.  Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.