The evolution and cultural transmission of percussive technology: integrating evidence from palaeoanthropology and primatology.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] T. Matsuzawa. Field experiments on use of stone tools by chimpanzees in the wild. , 1994 .
[2] W. McGrew,et al. Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia , 2006, Primates.
[3] N. Toth,et al. Making Silent Stones Speak: Human Evolution And The Dawn Of Technology , 1993 .
[4] C Boesch,et al. Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. , 1990, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.
[5] L. Eberly,et al. Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees , 2004, Nature.
[6] M. Tomasello. CHAPTER 15 – Do Apes Ape? , 1996 .
[7] R. V. S. Wright,et al. Imitative Learning of a Flaked Stone Technology—The Case of an Orangutan , 2009 .
[8] N. Toth. The oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts , 1985 .
[9] S. Perry. What Cultural Primatology Can Tell Anthropologists About the Evolution of Culture , 2006 .
[10] John W. Harris. The Karari industry, its place in East African prehistory , 1978 .
[11] W. McGrew,et al. Why Don't Chimpanzees in Gabon Crack Nuts? , 1997, International Journal of Primatology.
[12] T. P. Neufeld,et al. Direct Induction of Autophagy by Atg1 Inhibits Cell Growth and Induces Apoptotic Cell Death , 2007, Current Biology.
[13] Andrew Whiten,et al. Observational learning from tool using models by human-reared and mother-reared capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[14] H. D. Lumley,et al. Les sites préhistoriques de la région de Fejej, Sud-Omo, Éthiopie, dans leur contexte stratigraphique et paléontologique. , 2004 .
[15] K. Butzer,et al. After the Australopithecines : stratigraphy, ecology, and culture, change in the Middle Pleistocene , 1975 .
[16] C. Boesch,et al. The Chimpanzees of the Tai Forest : Behavioural Ecology and Evolution , 2000 .
[17] Michael Tomasello,et al. Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[18] Blandine Bril,et al. Stone knapping : the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour , 2005 .
[19] N. Toth,et al. An Experimental Investigation into the Nature of Faceted Limestone “Spheroids” in the Early Palaeolithic , 1997 .
[20] C. K. Brain. The Hunters or the Hunted?: An Introduction to African Cave Taphonomy , 1983, Geological Magazine.
[21] J. Pelegrin,et al. Remarks about archaeological techniques and methods of knapping : elements of a cognitive approach to stone knapping , 2005 .
[22] R. Blumenschine. Percussion marks, tooth marks, and experimental determinations of the timing of hominid and carnivore access to long bones at FLK Zinjanthropus, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania , 1995 .
[23] David Lordkipanidze,et al. Les industries lithiques préoldowayennes du début du Pléistocène inférieur du site de Dmanissi en Géorgie , 2005 .
[24] A. Whiten,et al. Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.
[25] David Reich,et al. Genetic Structure of Chimpanzee Populations , 2007, PLoS genetics.
[26] Thomas Wynn,et al. An Ape's View of the Oldowan , 1989 .
[27] Bethan J. Morgan,et al. Chimpanzees use stone hammers in Cameroon , 2006, Current Biology.
[28] Andrew Whiten,et al. Spread of arbitrary conventions among chimpanzees: a controlled experiment , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[29] P. Richerson,et al. Why Culture is Common, but Cultural Evolution is Rare , 1996 .
[30] C. Marean,et al. Late PlioceneHomoand Oldowan Tools from the Hadar Formation (Kada Hadar Member), Ethiopia , 1996 .
[31] W. Köhler. The Mentality of Apes. , 2018, Nature.
[32] C. Sanz,et al. New Insights into Chimpanzees, Tools, and Termites from the Congo Basin , 2004, The American Naturalist.
[33] B. Galef. The question of animal culture , 1992, Human nature.
[34] J. C. Redmond,et al. Early hominid brain evolution: a new look at old endocasts. , 2000, Journal of human evolution.
[35] C. R. Peters,et al. Landscape perspectives on possible land use patterns for Early Pleistocene hominids in the Olduvai Basin, Tanzania , 1995 .
[36] M. Tomasello,et al. Chimpanzee and Human Cultures , 1998, Current Anthropology.
[37] A. Whiten,et al. CHARTING CULTURAL VARIATION IN CHIMPANZEES , 2001 .
[38] F. Waal,et al. Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees , 2005, Primates.
[39] A. Whiten,et al. Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[40] P. Izar,et al. Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications , 2008 .
[41] C. K. Brain,et al. Breathing life into fossils : taphonomic studies in honor of C.K. (Bob) Brain , 2007 .
[42] A. Brooks,et al. Encyclopedia of Human Evolution and Prehistory , 1988 .
[43] Andrew Whiten,et al. Social learning of nut-cracking behavior in East African sanctuary-living chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.
[44] C. Brace,et al. Human origins : Louis Leakey and the East African evidence , 1977 .
[45] F. Brown,et al. Stratigraphic Relation between Lokalalei 1A and Lokalalei 2C, Pliocene Archaeological Sites in West Turkana, Kenya , 2002 .
[46] P. Mein,et al. Earliest man and environments in the lake rudolf basin , 1976 .
[47] Jody Hey,et al. Divergence population genetics of chimpanzees. , 2004, Molecular biology and evolution.
[48] Tim D. White,et al. Fejej: a new paleoanthropological research area in Ethiopia , 1991 .
[49] Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al. Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior , 2001, Springer Japan.
[50] Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo,et al. The Oldowan industry of Peninj and its bearing on the reconstruction of the technological skills of Lower Pleistocene hominids. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.
[51] E. Vrba,et al. Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old Bouri hominids. , 1999, Science.
[52] B. Wood,et al. Comparative context of Plio-Pleistocene hominin brain evolution. , 2001, Journal of human evolution.
[53] B. Latimer,et al. Australopithecus garhi: a new species of early hominid from Ethiopia. , 1999, Science.
[54] H. J. Jerison,et al. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence , 1973 .
[55] Gary T. Garufi,et al. Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool-making and Tool-using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1999 .
[56] M. Leakey,et al. Excavations in beds I and II, 1960-1963 , 1971 .
[57] Rafael Mora Torcal,et al. Oldowan, rather more than smashing stones , 2003 .
[58] D. Biro,et al. Emergence of a culture in wild chimpan-zees: education by master-apprenticeship , 2001 .
[59] T. White,et al. The earliest Acheulean from Konso-Gardula , 1992, Nature.
[60] M. Leakey. Cultural Patterns in the Olduvai Sequence , 1975 .
[61] Lydia M Hopper,et al. Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[62] S. Tebbich,et al. Do woodpecker finches acquire tool-use by social learning? , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[63] S. Antón,et al. Earliest Pleistocene hominid cranial remains from Dmanisi, Republic of Georgia: taxonomy, geological setting, and age. , 2000, Science.
[64] T. Matsuzawa,et al. How does stone-tool use emerge? Introduction of stones and nuts to naïve chimpanzees in captivity , 2005, Primates.
[65] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees , 2007, Animal Behaviour.
[66] C. Boesch,et al. Cultural differences in army ant predation by West African chimpanzees? A comparative study of microecological variables , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[67] N. Toth,et al. Microwear polishes on early stone tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya , 1981, Nature.
[68] T. Matsuzawa,et al. Ant‐dipping among the chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, and some comparisons with other sites , 2002, American journal of primatology.
[69] B. Nickel,et al. Demographic History and Genetic Differentiation in Apes , 2006, Current Biology.
[70] A. Kacelnik,et al. Behavioural ecology: Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows , 2005, Nature.
[71] S. Blackmore. The Meme Machine , 1999 .
[72] F. Howell,et al. Depositional environments, archeological occurrences and hominids from Members E and F of the Shungura Formation (Omo basin, Ethiopia) , 1987 .
[73] W. McGrew. Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution , 1992 .
[74] F. Spoor,et al. Implications of new early Homo fossils from Ileret, east of Lake Turkana, Kenya , 2007, Nature.
[75] K. Kuman,et al. DISCOVERY OF NEW ARTEFACTS AT KROMDRAAI , 1997 .
[76] H. Roche,et al. Les sites archéologiques plio-pléistocènes de la formation de Nachukui, West Turkana, Kenya , 1994 .
[77] R. Leakey,et al. Pliocene and Pleistocene archeological sites west of Lake Turkana, Kenya , 1992 .
[78] M. Sahnouni,et al. The Site of Ain Hanech Revisited: New Investigations at this Lower Pleistocene Site in Northern Algeria , 1998 .
[79] R. Klein,et al. Systematic Butchery by Plio/Pleistocene Hominids at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania [and Comments and Reply] , 1986, Current Anthropology.
[80] A. Whiten. Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1998, Journal of comparative psychology.
[81] E. Hovers. Treading Carefully : Site Formation Processes and Pliocene Lithic Technology , 2003 .
[82] R. Potts,et al. Current research on the late Pliocene and Pleistocene deposits north of Homa Mountain, southwestern Kenya. , 1999, Journal of human evolution.
[83] S. Lycett,et al. Phylogenetic analyses of behavior support existence of culture among wild chimpanzees , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[84] Christophe Boesch,et al. Is nut cracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour , 1994 .
[85] K. Kuman. The archaeology of Sterkfontein—past and present , 1994 .
[86] A. Vekua,et al. A Plio-Pleistocene hominid from Dmanisi, East Georgia, Caucasus , 1995, Nature.
[87] Nicholas Toth,et al. The Oldowan: The Tool Making of Early Hominins and Chimpanzees Compared , 2009 .
[88] M. Tomasello. Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees , 1990 .
[89] C. R. Peters,et al. Archaeological predictions for hominid land use in the paleo-Olduvai Basin, Tanzania, during lowermost Bed II times. , 1998, Journal of human evolution.
[90] G. Isaac,et al. Plio-pleistocene archaeology , 1997 .
[91] J. Pruetz,et al. Savanna Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, Hunt with Tools , 2007, Current Biology.
[92] M. Ruvolo,et al. The geographic apportionment of mitochondrial genetic diversity in east African chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. , 1997, Molecular biology and evolution.
[93] K. Kuman,et al. Stratigraphy, artefact industries and hominid associations for Sterkfontein, member 5. , 2000, Journal of human evolution.
[94] Brief communication: cutmarks on a plio-pleistocene hominid from Sterkfontein, South Africa. , 2000, American journal of physical anthropology.
[95] D. Biro,et al. Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments , 2003, Animal Cognition.
[96] P. Izar,et al. Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others’ tool-using skills , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[97] Yukimaru Sugiyama,et al. Social tradition and the use of tool‐composites by wild chimpanzees , 1997 .
[98] H. Thieme. Lower Palaeolithic hunting spears from Germany , 1997, Nature.
[99] P. Ditchfield,et al. Research on late Pliocene Oldowan sites at Kanjera South, Kenya. , 1999, Journal of human evolution.
[100] C. Boesch,et al. Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking , 1984, Primates.
[101] H. Roche,et al. Early hominid stone tool production and technical skill 2.34 Myr ago in West Turkana, Kenya , 1999, Nature.
[102] J. D. Heinzelin,et al. Récentes recherches dans le gisement oldowayen d'Ain Hanech, Algérie , 1996 .
[103] Andrew Whiten,et al. The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans , 2005, Nature.
[104] Sophie A. de Beaune,et al. The Invention of Technology. , 2004 .
[105] R. Sevcik,et al. Pan the Tool-Maker: Investigations into the Stone Tool-Making and Tool-Using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1993 .
[106] Michelle Y. Merrill,et al. Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture , 2003, Science.
[107] E. Visalberghi,et al. Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) to crack open palm nuts. , 2007, American journal of physical anthropology.
[108] A. Whiten,et al. Social diffusion of novel foraging methods in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[109] K. Dautenhahn,et al. Imitation in Animals and Artifacts , 2002 .
[110] Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al. Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees , 2021, Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology.
[111] E. Ottoni,et al. Semifree-ranging Tufted Capuchins (Cebus apella) Spontaneously Use Tools to Crack Open Nuts , 2001, International Journal of Primatology.
[112] K. Gibson,et al. “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives on ape “language” , 1990 .
[113] C. Heyes,et al. Social learning in animals : the roots of culture , 1996 .
[114] A. Whiten,et al. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[115] Hélène Roche,et al. Les sites archéologiques plio-pléistocènes de la formation de Nachukui, Ouest-Turkana, Kenya: bilan synthétique 1997-2001 , 2003 .
[116] P. C. Lee,et al. Capuchin Stone Tool Use in Caatinga Dry Forest , 2004, Science.
[117] B. Wood. Hominid cranial remains , 1991 .
[118] P. Renne,et al. 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona, Ethiopia , 1997, Nature.
[119] A. Whiten,et al. Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.
[120] Andrew Whiten,et al. Transmission of Multiple Traditions within and between Chimpanzee Groups , 2007, Current Biology.
[121] Andrew Whiten,et al. The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[122] A. Whiten,et al. How do apes ape? , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[123] M. Domínguez‐Rodrigo,et al. The ST Site Complex at Peninj, West Lake Natron, Tanzania: Implications for Early Hominid Behavioural Models , 2002 .
[124] R. Lewin,et al. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind , 1994 .
[125] W. McGrew,et al. The Cultured Chimpanzee: Reflections on Cultural Primatology , 2004 .
[126] J. Kitahara-Frisch,et al. The acquisition of stone-tool use in captive chimpanzees , 1985, Primates.
[127] P. R. Jones,et al. Experimental Implement Manufacture and Use; A Case Study from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania , 1981 .
[128] Michael J. Rogers,et al. 2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.
[129] Dorothy Fragaszy,et al. Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools , 2004, American journal of primatology.
[130] C. Egeland,et al. Deconstructing Olduvai: A Taphonomic Study of the Bed I Sites , 2007 .
[131] N. Toth,et al. The cutting edge : new approaches to the archaeology of human origins , 2009 .
[132] S. Boinski,et al. Substrate and Tool Use by Brown Capuchins in Suriname: Ecological Contexts and Cognitive Bases , 2000 .
[133] T. Plummer. Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. , 2004, American journal of physical anthropology.
[134] T. White,et al. The first skull of Australopithecus boisei , 1997, Nature.
[135] C. Boesch,et al. 4,300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[136] C. Sanz,et al. Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. , 2007, Journal of human evolution.
[137] E. Visalberghi,et al. Do monkeys ape?: ten years after , 2002 .
[138] A. Whiten,et al. On the Nature and Evolution of Imitation in the Animal Kingdom: Reappraisal of a Century of Research , 1992 .
[139] T. Humle. Ant Dipping in Chimpanzees: An Example of How Microecological Variables, Tool Use, and Culture Reflect the Cognitive Abilities of Chimpanzees , 2006 .
[140] Bernard Wood,et al. Older than the Oldowan? Rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use , 2003 .
[141] Lawrence H. Keeley,et al. A Reappraisal of the Clacton Spearpoint , 1977, Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society.
[142] S. Lycett,et al. Cladistic analyses of behavioural variation in wild Pan troglodytes: exploring the chimpanzee culture hypothesis. , 2009, Journal of human evolution.
[143] Peter Jones. Experimental butchery with modern stone tools and its relevance for Palaeolithic archaeology , 1980 .
[144] Anne Delagnes,et al. Late Pliocene hominid knapping skills: the case of Lokalalei 2C, West Turkana, Kenya. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.
[145] B. Wood,et al. Early archaeological sites, hominid remains and traces of fire from Chesowanja, Kenya , 1981, Nature.