Talking About Behaviors in the Passive Voice Increases Task Performance

Summary: Self-talk can help people redirect their attention focused on themselves to the tasks they are working on with important consequences for their task performance. Across four experiments and two different types of languages, Turkish and Slovak, people describing their own behaviors to themselves, as well as merely reading or writing sentences depicting some fictitious events, in the passive (vs. the active) voice performed better on various tasks of motor and verbal performance. The effect was present to the extent that people maintained their control over task-distracting thoughts or felt more responsible for their task success/failure. In sum, talking about task behaviors in the passive voice may increase the perceived role of task-related factors while decreasing the role of agent-related factors in achieving task success, whereby the task focus, hence performance, increases. The results are important for understanding the role of self-talk in performance with implications for changing important outcomes. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. After it was first proposed by L.S. Vygotsky (1934/1987), numerous studies have shown the fundamental role played by self-talk in the control of thoughts and behaviors (e.g., Winsler, Fernyhough, & Montero, 2009). In the present investigation, we focus on self-talk about task performance. We ask if the grammatical structure of self-talk can give us a clue about its role in improving task performance. Can, for example, a subtle difference in the way in which we linguistically think of our behaviors as the causes of task success, such as the difference between ‘I will do it’ versus ‘It will be done’, change our task performance? If yes, we can not only better understand the phenomenon of self-talk as it links language and behavior but also improve behavioral performance in various domains of psychology including athletic performance, work, school, health, and clinical contexts. Self-talk can improve task performance by directing people’s attention either to themselves or to the tasks they may be working on. In athletic performance settings, for example, athletes use self-talk for two basic purposes: selfinstruction and motivation (for a review, see, e.g., Hardy, 2006). The self-instructional self-talk relates to the technical aspects of tasks (e.g., ‘I see the target’) whereas the motivational self-talk focuses on people’s own role in these tasks (e.g., ‘I can do it’). Given the beneficial effects of both types of self-talk on task performance (Hardy, Gammage, & Hall, 2001), it becomes critical to understand how agents versus tasks can be differentially highlighted via language.

[1]  Ethan Kross,et al.  Enhancing the Pace of Recovery , 2008, Psychological science.

[2]  D. Wegner,et al.  Chronic thought suppression. , 1994, Journal of personality.

[3]  Michael G. Johnson Sytatic position and rated meaning , 1967 .

[4]  G. Bohner,et al.  Writing about rape: use of the passive voice and other distancing text features as an expression of perceived responsibility of the victim. , 2001, The British journal of social psychology.

[5]  N. Zourbanos,et al.  Self-Talk and Sports Performance , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[6]  J. Hardy Speaking clearly: A critical review of the self-talk literature. , 2006 .

[7]  Cindy K. Chung,et al.  The Psychological Functions of Function Words , 2007 .

[8]  J. K. Bock Syntactic persistence in language production , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  J. Brockner Self-esteem, self-consciousness, and task performance: Replications, extensions, and possible explanations. , 1979 .

[10]  C. Jung THE COLLECTED WORKS OF , 2014 .

[11]  Martha E. Francis,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Linguistic Predictors of Adaptive Bereavement , 2022 .

[12]  Bettina Seipp Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings , 1991 .

[13]  M. Harris Noun Animacy and the Passive Voice: A Developmental Approach , 1978 .

[14]  Stephen J. Blumberg,et al.  The white bear suppression inventory: revisiting its factor structure , 2000 .

[15]  Dolores Albarracín,et al.  Splitting of the Mind , 2012, Social psychological and personality science.

[16]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Marianne Jones,et al.  Private Speech, Executive Functioning, and the Development of Verbal Self-Regulation , 2009 .

[18]  R. Weinberg,et al.  The Effects of Motivational versus Instructional Self-Talk on Improving Motor Performance , 2000 .

[19]  Pessimistic self-preoccupation, performance deficits, and depression. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  David Crystal,et al.  The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language , 2012, Modern Language Review.

[21]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Language use of depressed and depression-vulnerable college students , 2004 .

[22]  Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al.  Is Syntax Separate or Shared Between Languages , 2004 .

[23]  R. Hembree Correlates, Causes, Effects, and Treatment of Test Anxiety , 1988 .

[24]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  The Secret Life of Pronouns , 2003, Psychological science.

[25]  Ulrich Müller,et al.  Interference control in a new rule use task: age-related changes, labeling, and attention. , 2004, Child development.

[26]  M. Bradley,et al.  Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. , 1994, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[27]  M. Pickering,et al.  Is Syntax Separate or Shared Between Languages? , 2004, Psychological science.

[28]  C. Carver,et al.  Reassertion and giving up: The interactive role of self-directed attention and outcome expectancy. , 1979 .

[29]  D. Mackinnon,et al.  Equivalence of the Mediation, Confounding and Suppression Effect , 2000, Prevention Science.

[30]  Adam Winsler,et al.  "Should I let them talk?": Private speech and task performance among preschool children with and without behavior problems , 2007 .

[31]  J. Coyne,et al.  Learned helplessness as experimenter-induced failure and its alleviation with attentional redeployment. , 1980, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[32]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[33]  C. Hall,et al.  A descriptive study of athlete self-talk. , 2001 .

[34]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[35]  Michael B. Hein,et al.  The Self-Talk Scale: Development, Factor Analysis, and Validation , 2009, Journal of personality assessment.