Body representations and brain damage

We review changes in body representation in patients with brain hemisphere damage and discuss their relationship with impaired limb movements in peripersonal space, navigation between objects/obstacles and control of the body's general posture and balance. The egocentric representation of the body's median sagittal axis (considered as the main zone around which movements are anchored) has been studied in most detail. This reference is distorted in patients with spatial neglect and involves a combination of ipsilesional translation and contralesional tilt. There are clear links with the patients' difficulties in egocentric tasks, activities of daily living and postural control. In both healthy subjects and patients, this reference axis can be modulated by somaesthetic, vestibular and visual stimulations; these phenomena have been used in rehabilitation programmes to reduce disease-induced deviations. A few studies have analyzed other lateral body reference (at the shoulders, in particular). These references were found to be more severely affected than the body midline (notably on the contralesional side). The severity of the distortion was related to the presence of lesions that mainly affected the parietal, somatosensory and multimodal association cortex (notably around the intraparietal sulcus) and, to a lesser extent, the middle temporal and frontal dorsolateral premotor cortex. These convergent results suggested that patients (notably those with neglect) have a complex distortion of the body schema and the perceptive representations of the body, that does not simply correspond to poor awareness of the contralateral hemicorpus.

[1]  D. Perani,et al.  Unilateral neglect: Personal and extra-personal , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  O. Blanke,et al.  The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans , 2011, Brain Research Reviews.

[3]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[4]  M Rousseaux,et al.  Where is the “straight ahead” in spatial neglect? , 2006, Neurology.

[5]  Time since stroke influences the impact of hemianopia and spatial neglect on visual-spatial tasks. , 2012, Neuropsychology.

[6]  I. Keller,et al.  Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation Improves Arm Position Sense in Spatial Neglect , 2013, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[7]  D. Pérennou,et al.  Postural disorders and spatial neglect in stroke patients: a strong association. , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[8]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Space Coding in Primate Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.

[9]  Alessandro Farnè,et al.  In search of biased egocentric reference frames in neglect , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  O. Schillaci,et al.  Cerebral plasticity in acute vestibular deficit , 2009, European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology.

[11]  G. Spidalieri,et al.  Psychophysical properties of the trunk midline. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Shaun Gallagher,et al.  Body image and body schema: A conceptual clarification. , 1986 .

[13]  Pia Rotshtein,et al.  Separating neural correlates of allocentric and egocentric neglect: Distinct cortical sites and common white matter disconnections , 2010, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  J. Honoré,et al.  Is there a distortion of body projection in extracorporeal space in neglect patients? , 2000, Neuroreport.

[15]  A. Kastrup,et al.  Prism Adaptation Improves Ego-Centered but Not Allocentric Neglect in Early Rehabilitation Patients , 2013, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[16]  M. Fabri,et al.  Bilateral receptive fields and callosal connectivity of the body midline representation in the first somatosensory area of primates. , 1986, Somatosensory research.

[17]  K M Heilman,et al.  Bodily neglect and orientational biases in unilateral neglect syndrome and normal subjects , 1990, Neurology.

[18]  J. Honoré,et al.  Neuroanatomy of space, body, and posture perception in patients with right hemisphere stroke , 2013, Neurology.

[19]  F. Vignemont Body schema and body image—Pros and cons , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  D. Pérennou,et al.  Contribution of Each Lower Limb to Upright Standing in Stroke Patients , 2008, Stroke.

[22]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  J. Babinski,et al.  Contribution a l'etude des troubles mentaux dans l'hemiplegie organique cerebrale (anosognosie) , 1914 .

[24]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[25]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Eye movements tell only half the story , 2003, Neurology.

[26]  B. Amblard,et al.  Understanding the pusher behavior of some stroke patients with spatial deficits: a pilot study. , 2002, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[27]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Spatial Compression in Visual Neglect: A Case Study , 1991, Cortex.

[28]  Michael Fetter,et al.  The interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation to subjective “straight ahead” orientation in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  J. Honoré,et al.  Where is straight ahead to a patient with unilateral vestibular loss? , 2013, Cortex.

[30]  D Boisson,et al.  Predominance of postural imbalance in left hemiparetic patients. , 1997, Scandinavian journal of rehabilitation medicine.

[31]  G. Rode,et al.  Postural asymmetry reduction by vestibular caloric stimulation in left hemiparetic patients. , 1998, Scandinavian journal of rehabilitation medicine.

[32]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  E. Làdavas,et al.  Neglect treatment by prism adaptation: What recovers and for how long , 2007, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[34]  P Rougier,et al.  Asymmetric standing posture after stroke is related to a biased egocentric coordinate system , 2009, Neurology.

[35]  Sylvie Chokron,et al.  Egocentric frame of reference: its role in spatial biasafter right hemisphere lesions , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Remission of anosognosia for right hemiplegia and neglect after caloric vestibular stimulation. , 2013, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[37]  A. Berthoz,et al.  The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  J. Sanes,et al.  Spatial coding of visual and somatic sensory information in body‐centred coordinates , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  J. Honoré,et al.  Hemianopia and Neglect Influence on Straight-Ahead Perception , 2010, European Neurology.

[40]  J. Honoré,et al.  The egocentric reference deviation of neglect patients is influenced by visuospatial attention , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  C. Butter,et al.  Ipsilesional Displacement of Egocentric Midline in Neglect Patients with, but Not in Those Without, Extensive Right Parietal Damage , 1997 .

[42]  M. Rusconi,et al.  Long-Term Efficacy of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect: Preliminary Results on Different Spatial Components , 2012, TheScientificWorldJournal.

[43]  J. Honoré,et al.  The pusher syndrome reverses the orienting bias caused by spatial neglect , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Spatial hemineglect in back space. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  A fronto-parietal system for computing the egocentric spatial frame of reference in humans , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  Gianna Cocchini,et al.  The Fluff Test: A simple task to assess body representation neglect , 2001 .

[47]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The Directional Coding of Reaching Movements. A Visuomotor Conception of Spatial Neglect , 1987 .

[48]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Evidence for a Disturbance of the Body Schema in Neglect , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[49]  S. Aglioti,et al.  The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[50]  H. Karnath,et al.  Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  K. Heilman,et al.  Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.

[52]  K. Heilman,et al.  Material specific hemispherical arousal , 1976, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  J. Honoré,et al.  Mislocalization of tactile stimuli applied to the trunk in spatial neglect , 2013, Cortex.

[54]  Francis R. Loayza,et al.  Right parietal dominance in spatial egocentric discrimination , 2011, NeuroImage.

[55]  J. Honoré,et al.  Reducing rightward bias of subjective straight ahead in neglect patients by changes in body orientation , 2008, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[56]  S. Ferber,et al.  Parietal and occipital lobe contributions to perception of straight ahead orientation , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[57]  Marc Rousseaux,et al.  Straight ahead in spatial neglect , 2004, Neurology.

[58]  M Rousseaux,et al.  Validation of a standardized assessment of postural control in stroke patients: the Postural Assessment Scale for Stroke Patients (PASS). , 1999, Stroke.

[59]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Neuroanatomical Dissections of Unilateral Visual Neglect Symptoms: ALE Meta-Analysis of Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[60]  G. Vallar,et al.  Gravity and hemineglect , 1995, Neuroreport.

[61]  Umberto Sabatini,et al.  Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  J. Honoré,et al.  Functional neuro-anatomy of egocentric versus allocentric space representation , 2014, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[63]  Eve Dupierrix,et al.  Long Lasting Egocentric Disorientation Induced by Normal Sensori-Motor Spatial Interaction , 2009, PloS one.

[64]  P Bartolomeo,et al.  [Presenting a battery for assessing spatial neglect. Norms and effects of age, educational level, sex, hand and laterality]. , 2001, Revue neurologique.

[65]  W. Heide,et al.  Acute hemianopic patients do not show a contralesional deviation in the line bisection task , 2009, Journal of Neurology.

[66]  G. Kerkhoff,et al.  Convergent and divergent effects of neck proprioceptive and visual motion stimulation on visual space processing in neglect , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  F. Patria,et al.  Psychophysical Properties of Line Bisection and Body Midline Perception in Unilateral Neglect , 2000, Cortex.

[68]  G. Holmes,et al.  Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions , 1911 .

[69]  G. Hörnsten Constant error of visual egocentric orientation in patients with acute vestibular disorder. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Meta-analytical definition and functional connectivity of the human vestibular cortex , 2012, NeuroImage.

[71]  J. Driver,et al.  Prism adaptation aftereffects in stroke patients with spatial neglect: Pathological effects on subjective straight ahead but not visual open-loop pointing , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  V. Nougier,et al.  Role of gravity-based information on the orientation and localization of the perceived body midline , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[73]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[74]  H. Karnath,et al.  The perception of body orientation after neck-proprioceptive stimulation , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[75]  M. Gresty,et al.  Perception of longitudinal body axis in patients with stroke: a pilot study , 2006, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[76]  K M Heilman,et al.  Performance On hemispatial pointing task by patients with neglect syndrome , 1983, Neurology.

[77]  M. Imbert,et al.  Variations of the egocentric reference among normal subjects and a patient with unilateral neglect , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[78]  H. Karnath,et al.  Optokinetic stimulation influences the disturbed perception of body orientation in spatial neglect. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[79]  J. Honoré,et al.  Plantar stimulation can affect subjective straight-ahead in neglect patients , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[80]  Sylvie Chokron,et al.  Patterns of dissociation between left hemineglect and deviation of the egocentric reference , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Beyond unilateral neglect. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[82]  J. Pélissier,et al.  Posture et équilibre après accident vasculaire cérébral : désavantage de l'hémisphère droit , 2008 .

[83]  N. Smania,et al.  Visuomotor imagery and rehabilitation of neglect. , 1997, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[84]  J. Honoré,et al.  Straight-Ahead Pointing Correlates with Long-Line Bisection in Neglect Patients , 2004, Cortex.