Line bisection as a neural marker of approach motivation.

Approach motivation has been reliably associated with relative left prefrontal brain activity as measured with electroencephalography (EEG). Motivation researchers have increasingly used the line bisection task, a behavioral measure of relative cerebral asymmetry, as a neural index of approach motivation-related processes. Despite its wide adoption, however, the line bisection task has not been confirmed as a valid measure of the precise pattern of activity linked to approach motivation. In two studies, we demonstrate that line bisection bias is specifically related to baseline, approach-related, prefrontal EEG alpha asymmetry (Study 1) and is heightened by the same situational factors that heighten the same approach-related prefrontal EEG alpha asymmetry (Study 2). Results support the line bisection task as an efficient and unobtrusive behavioral neuroscience measure of approach motivation.

[1]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Proneness to hypomania/mania symptoms or depression symptoms and asymmetrical frontal cortical responses to an anger-evoking event. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Space and the parietal cortex , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Behavioral activation sensitivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: covariation of putative indicators related to risk for mood disorders. , 1997, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[4]  David M. Amodio,et al.  Implicit regulatory focus associated with asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. , 2004 .

[5]  C. Geula,et al.  Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. , 1993, Archives of neurology.

[6]  A. Milner,et al.  To halve and to halve not: An analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Hagemann,et al.  Individual differences in anterior EEG asymmetry: methodological problems and solutions , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[8]  Leonard L. Martin,et al.  Stereotyping, Self-Affirmation, and the Cerebral Hemispheres , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[9]  D. J. Lee Society and the Adolescent Self-Image , 1969 .

[10]  Jeffrey B. Henriques,et al.  Frontal Brain Asymmetry and Reward Responsiveness , 2005, Psychological science.

[11]  D. Pandya,et al.  Association fibre pathways of the brain: parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  J. Kuhl,et al.  Left-Hemispheric Activation and Self-Infiltration: Testing a Neuropsychological Model of Internalization , 2005 .

[13]  M. Gailliot,et al.  Ideological and Personal Zeal Reactions to Threat Among People With High Self-Esteem: Motivated Promotion Focus , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[14]  E. Higgins,et al.  Beyond pleasure and pain. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[15]  Matthew T. Sutherland,et al.  Recovery of correlated neuronal sources from EEG: The good and bad ways of using SOBI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[16]  Nira Liberman,et al.  The effect of global versus local processing styles on assimilation versus contrast in social judgment. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  E. Harmon-Jones Early Career Award. Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[18]  A. Elliot Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation , 2008 .

[19]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral activation and inhibition systems. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[21]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  E. Harmon-Jones Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[23]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Threat, high self-esteem, and reactive approach-motivation: Electroencephalographic evidence , 2009 .

[24]  R. E. Wheeler,et al.  Individual differences in anterior brain asymmetry and fundamental dimensions of emotion. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Cameron Anderson,et al.  Power, Approach, and Inhibition , 2003 .

[26]  Denise C. Marigold,et al.  Defensive zeal and the uncertain self: what makes you so sure? , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  R. A. Drake,et al.  Visual attention, emotion, and action tendency: Feeling active or passive , 2006 .

[29]  M. Rosenberg Society and the adolescent self-image , 1966 .

[30]  H. Duffau,et al.  Direct Evidence for a Parietal-Frontal Pathway Subserving Spatial Awareness in Humans , 2005, Science.

[31]  I. Mcgregor,et al.  Offensive Defensiveness: Toward an Integrative Neuroscience of Compensatory Zeal After Mortality Salience, Personal Uncertainty, and Other Poignant Self-Threats , 2006 .

[32]  Jens Förster,et al.  Effects of motivational cues on perceptual asymmetry: implications for creativity and analytical problem solving. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Approach-Motivated Positive Affect Reduces Breadth of Attention , 2008, Psychological science.