Is it important that the mind is in a body? Inhibition and the heart.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] A. Noma,et al. Reconstruction of sino-atrial node pacemaker potential based on the voltage clamp experiments. , 1980, The Japanese journal of physiology.
[3] G. Logan. On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .
[4] M. W. Molen,et al. How are tonic and phasic cardiovascular changes related to central motor command? , 1993, Biological Psychology.
[5] Anders Kjellberg,et al. Orienting Activity in Two-Stimulus Paradigms as Reflected in Heart Rate , 2021, The Orienting Reflex in Humans.
[6] Beatrice C. Lacey,et al. Studies of heart rate and other bodily processes in sensorimotor behavior. , 1974 .
[7] D. Fowles. Psychophysiology and psychopathology: a motivational approach. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[8] D. Powell,et al. Autonomic responses are elicited by electrical stimulation of the medial but not lateral frontal cortex in rabbits , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.
[9] D. Meyer,et al. The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] G. Mandler. Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress , 1984 .
[11] Henning Scheich,et al. Classical conditioning of tone-signaled bradycardia modifies 2-deoxyglucose uptake patterns in cortex, thalamus, habenula, caudate-putamen and hippocampal formation , 1986, Brain Research.
[12] G. Logan,et al. In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] D. McCloskey,et al. Cardiovascular and respiratory responses to changes in central command during isometric exercise at constant muscle tension , 1972, The Journal of physiology.
[14] C. C. Wood,et al. Cardiac cycle time effects on performance, phasic cardiac responses, and their intercorrelation in choice reaction time. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[15] M. W. van der Molen,et al. On the shift from anticipatory heart rate deceleration to acceleratory recovery: revisiting the role of response factors. , 1990, Psychophysiology.
[16] R. Wilcott. Prefrontal cortex and bulbar reticular formation and behavioral inhibition in the Rat , 1984, Brain Research Bulletin.
[17] D. H. Vandercar,et al. Lateral subthalamic area as mediator of classically conditioned bradycardia in rabbits. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.
[18] J. Gray,et al. Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[19] J. Jennings,et al. Recall, recognition, and rate: memory and the heart. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[20] M. Lambertz,et al. Reticular formation of the lower brainstem. A common system for cardio-respiratory and somatomotor functions. Considerations aided by computer simulations. , 1985, Journal of the autonomic nervous system.
[21] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[22] S Kornblum,et al. Does motor programming necessitate response execution? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] M. Coles. Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[24] G. Mogenson,et al. Limbic-motor integration , 1987 .
[25] D I Boomsma,et al. Does the heart know what the eye sees? A cardiac/pupillometric analysis of motor preparation and response execution. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[26] M. W. van der Molen,et al. Response inhibition initiates cardiac deceleration: evidence from a sensory-motor compatibility paradigm. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[27] G. L. Gebber,et al. Differential control of sympathetic nerve discharge by the brain stem. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.
[28] Beatrice C. Lacey,et al. Change in heart period: A function of sensorimotor event timing within the cardiac cycle , 1977 .
[29] M. W. van der Molen,et al. Behavioral modulation patterns fit an animal model of vagus-cardiac pacemaker interactions. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[30] Beatrice C. Lacey,et al. Cognitive Modulation of Time‐Dependent Primary Bradycardia , 1980 .
[31] J. Gray,et al. Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. , 1990 .
[32] M. W. van der Molen,et al. On the synchrony of stopping motor responses and delaying heartbeats. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] E. Rolls. A Theory of Emotion, and its Application to Understanding the Neural Basis of Emotion , 1990 .
[34] R. Lazarus,et al. Some parameters of heart rate change: perceptual versus motor task requirements, noxiousness, and uncertainty. , 1970, Psychophysiology.
[35] B C Lacey,et al. Two-way communication between the heart and the brain. Significance of time within the cardiac cycle. , 1978, The American psychologist.
[36] R. Llinás. The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of mammalian neurons: insights into central nervous system function. , 1988, Science.
[37] Fritz Drasgow,et al. New time-series statistic for detecting rhythmic co-occurrence in the frequency domain: the weighted coherence and its application to psychophysiological research. , 1980 .
[38] J. Beatty,et al. Response selection and initiation in speeded reactions: a pupillometric analysis. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] P. Obrist. Cardiovascular Psychophysiology: A Perspective , 1981 .
[40] M. Bradley,et al. Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. , 1990, Psychological review.