Jeer Pressure: The Behavioral Effects of Observing Ridicule of Others

Two experiments examined “jeer pressure,” which is a hypothesized inhibiting effect of observing another person being ridiculed. Jeer pressure was expected to induce conformity to others’ opinions; concern about failing or standing out; and conventional, uncreative thinking. In both experiments, participants observed videotapes containing either other-ridiculing humor, self-ridiculing humor, or nonridiculing or no humor. Participants then completed tasks that assessed conformity, fear of failure, and creativity. Results of both experiments showed that participants who viewed ridicule of others were more conforming and more afraid of failing than were those who viewed self-ridicule or no ridicule. Creativity was not influenced by the humor manipulation. Experiment 2 also included a lexical decision task to assess whether salience of potential rejection mediated the obtained behavioral effects. Salience of rejection mediated the effects of humor on fear of failure but not the effects of humor on conformity.

[1]  J. M. Olson,et al.  The (null) effects of exposure to disparagement humor on stereotypes and attitudes , 1999 .

[2]  J. M. Olson,et al.  Telling Jokes That Disparage Social Groups: Effects on the Joke Teller's Stereotypes' , 1997 .

[3]  W. F. Sharkey Why Would Anyone Want to Intentionally Embarrass me , 1997 .

[4]  Robin M. Kowalski,et al.  Aversive Interpersonal Behaviors , 1997 .

[5]  M. Baldwin,et al.  Self-esteem and "if . . . then" contingencies of interpersonal acceptance. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  W. F. Sharkey Use and responses to intentional embarrassment , 1992 .

[7]  Richard M. Sorrentino,et al.  Risk-taking in games of chance and skill: Informational and affective influences on choice behavior. , 1992 .

[8]  Roy F. Baumeister,et al.  A Three-Component Model of Children's Teasing: Aggression, Humor, and Ambiguity , 1991 .

[9]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. , 1990 .

[10]  J. Campbell,et al.  Informational and normative routes to conformity: The effect of faction size as a function of norm extremity and attention to the stimulus. , 1989 .

[11]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  J. Bryant,et al.  CHILDREN'S IMITATION OF A RIDICULED MODEL , 1983 .

[13]  J. Suls Cognitive Processes in Humor Appreciation , 1983 .

[14]  J. Bryant,et al.  Ridicule as an Educational Corrective. , 1981 .

[15]  T. Wills Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology , 1981 .

[16]  L. Wheeler,et al.  Review of personality and social psychology , 1980 .

[17]  Christopher P. Wilson,et al.  Jokes: Form, content, use, and function , 1979 .

[18]  E. E. Jones,et al.  Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in response to noncontingent success. , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[20]  Effects of Humorous Disparagement of Self, Friend, and Enemy , 1976 .

[21]  W. Martineau,et al.  Chapter 5 – A Model of the Social Functions of Humor , 1972 .

[22]  前田 豊 "Pudd'nhead Wilson"について , 1970 .

[23]  D. J. Lee Society and the Adolescent Self-Image , 1969 .

[24]  E. Paul Torrance,et al.  Guiding creative talent , 1962 .

[25]  J. W. Atkinson,et al.  Achievement motive and test anxiety conceived as motive to approach success and motive to avoid failure. , 1960, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[26]  J. Guilford The structure of intellect. , 1956, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  W. V. Bingham,et al.  Expectancies , 1953 .

[28]  S. Freud,et al.  Jokes and Their Relation to the Unconscious , 1905 .