Wearable and automotive systems for affect recognition from physiology

Novel systems and algorithms have been designed and built to recognize affective patterns in physiological signals. Experiments were conducted for evaluation of the new systems and algorithms in three types of settings: a highly constrained laboratory setting, a largely unconstrained ambulatory environment, and a less unconstrained automotive environment. The laboratory experiment was designed to test for the presence of unique physiological patterns given a relatively motionless seated subject, intentionally expressing one of eight different emotions. This experiment generated a large dataset of physiological signals containing many day-today variations, and the proposed features contributed to a success rate of 81% for discriminating all eight emotions and up to 100% for subsets of emotion based on similar emotion qualities. For experiments in a largely unconstrained ambulatory setting, new wearable computer systems and sensors were developed and tested on subjects who walked, jogged, talked, and otherwise went about daily activities. Although physical motion often overwhelmed affective signals in this context, the systems developed in this thesis are currently useful as activity monitors, providing an image diary correlated with physiological signals. A third experiment was conducted in the natural but physically constrained environment of an automobile, generating a large database of physiological signals covering over 40 hours of driving data. This experiment was designed to induce varying amounts of stress in three conditions: rest, city diving and highway driving. Algorithms for detecting driver stress achieved a recognition rates of 96% for stress ratings based on task conditions and 89% accuracy using ratings of perceived stress from driver questionnaires. The results of second by second video coding of stressors by independent coders showed highly significant correlations with physiological features (up to r = .77 for over 4000 samples). Together, these three experiments show a range of success in recognizing affect from physiology, highlighting the need for more automatic context sensing in unconstrained conditions, and showing high recognition rates given somewhat constrained conditions. The recognition rates obtained thus far lend support to the hypothesis that many emotional differences can be automatically discriminated in patterns of physiological changes. (Copies available exclusively from MIT Libraries, Rm. 14-0551, Cambridge, MA 02139-4307. Ph. 617-253-5668; Fax 617-253-1690.)

[1]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Temperament, affective style, and frontal lobe asymmetry. , 1994 .

[2]  M. Helander Applicability of drivers' electrodermal response to the design of the traffic environment. , 1978, The Journal of applied psychology.

[3]  W. Cannon The James-Lange theory of emotions: a critical examination and an alternative theory. By Walter B. Cannon, 1927. , 1927, American Journal of Psychology.

[4]  Thoams Guthrie Zimmerman Personal area networks (PAN) : near-field intra-body communication , 1995 .

[5]  Sean A. Spence,et al.  Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[6]  P. Ekman,et al.  Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. , 1983, Science.

[7]  S. Schachter The Interaction of Cognitive and Physiological Determinants of Emotional State , 1964 .

[8]  H. Itoh,et al.  Young borderline hypertensives are hyperreactive to mental arithmetic stress: spectral analysis of R-R intervals. , 1995, Journal of the autonomic nervous system.

[9]  P. Ekman,et al.  Facial action coding system: a technique for the measurement of facial movement , 1978 .

[10]  H. Nagaraja,et al.  Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Richard O. Duda,et al.  Pattern classification and scene analysis , 1974, A Wiley-Interscience publication.

[12]  Elias Vyzas Recognition of emotional and cognitive states using physiological data , 1999 .

[13]  R. Levenson Autonomic Nervous System Differences among Emotions , 1992 .

[14]  P Kuronen,et al.  Heart rate responses to real and simulated BA Hawk MK 51 flight. , 1997, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[15]  H. V. van Geijn,et al.  Heart Rate Variability , 1993, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[16]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals. , 1990, The American psychologist.

[17]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Principles of psychophysiology : physical, social, and inferential elements , 1990 .

[18]  P. Venables,et al.  Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. , 1971, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Charles W. Therrien,et al.  Discrete Random Signals and Statistical Signal Processing , 1992 .

[20]  John L. Sibert,et al.  Heart rate variability: indicator of user state as an aid to human-computer interaction , 1998, CHI.

[21]  P. Ekman Facial expression and emotion. , 1993, The American psychologist.

[22]  R. Cohen,et al.  Power spectrum analysis of heart rate fluctuation: a quantitative probe of beat-to-beat cardiovascular control. , 1981, Science.

[23]  P. Ekman An argument for basic emotions , 1992 .

[24]  Anil K. Jain,et al.  Feature Selection: Evaluation, Application, and Small Sample Performance , 1997, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..

[25]  Andrew Ortony,et al.  The Cognitive Structure of Emotions , 1988 .

[26]  R. Krauss,et al.  Facial and autonomic manifestations of the dimensional structure of emotion , 1984 .

[27]  H. Selye Selye's Guide to Stress Research , 1980 .

[28]  M. Bradley,et al.  Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[29]  D. Goldstein,et al.  Sympathoadrenal medullary system and stress. , 1988, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[30]  D. Lykken,et al.  Correcting psychophysiological measures for individual differences in range. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  J. Russell,et al.  The psychology of facial expression: Studies in Emotion and Social Interaction , 1997 .

[32]  P. Lang International Affective Picture System (IAPS) : Technical Manual and Affective Ratings , 1995 .

[33]  A. Dale Magoun,et al.  Decision, estimation and classification , 1989 .

[34]  P. Lang The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[35]  R. Mccraty,et al.  The effects of emotions on short-term power spectrum analysis of heart rate variability . , 1995, The American journal of cardiology.

[36]  J Aasman,et al.  Operator Effort and the Measurement of Heart-Rate Variability , 1987, Human factors.

[37]  S. Hofmann,et al.  Ambulatory psychophysiological monitoring: A potentially useful tool when treating panic relapse * , 1996 .

[38]  Jennifer Healey,et al.  Augmented Reality through Wearable Computing , 1997, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[39]  A. Damasio Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .

[40]  J. Russell A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .

[41]  M. Schlossberg Introduction to Physiological Psychology , 1892, Nature.

[42]  P. Ekman Are there basic emotions? , 1992, Psychological review.

[43]  A. Ax The Physiological Differentiation between Fear and Anger in Humans , 1953, Psychosomatic medicine.

[44]  Manfred Clynes,et al.  Sentics: The touch of emotions , 1977 .

[45]  Charles F. Levinthal Introduction to Physiological Psychology , 1983 .

[46]  Rosalind W. PicardThe Ooine and Online Recognition of Emotion Expression from Physiological Data Category: Emotion Recognition , 1999 .