Assessing stereomotion thresholds with a high-resolution computer monitor

We investigated the feasibility of a computer-graphics-based method of assessing stereomotion thresholds (Silicon Graphics Stereoview stereoscopic system). Stereomotion thresholds for a rectangle oscillating in depth were determined with the use of a dual randomly interleaved staircase design. In a group of 31 naive observers, the average thresholds of 5.97′ of arc forcrossed stereomotion and 6.00′ of arc foruncrossed stereomotion were comparable to those assessed in earlier work done with optics-based techniques. By assessing the thresholds for a rectangle that was defined either by lateral motion or by changing size, in a group of experienced observers, we were able to show that any potential residual translational motion present in the display would not have influenced the stereomotion thresholds. Our findings suggest that this computer-graphics-based technique may be a reasonable alternative to optics-based methods of assessing stereomotion thresholds.

[1]  D Regan,et al.  Correlations between visual test results and flying performance on the advanced simulator for pilot training (ASPT). , 1981, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[2]  Whitman Richards,et al.  Response functions for sine- and square-wave modulations of disparity. , 1972 .

[3]  R. Reinecke,et al.  A reconsideration of amblyopia screening and stereopsis. , 1974, American journal of ophthalmology.

[4]  D Regan,et al.  The dissociation of sideways movements from movements in depth: psychophysics. , 1973, Vision research.

[5]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  J. Walraven,et al.  Amblyopia screening with random-dot stereograms. , 1975, American journal of ophthalmology.

[7]  J. Frisby,et al.  12 – THE PERCEPTION OF STEREODEPTH AND STEREOMOTION: Cortical Mechanisms , 1990 .

[8]  D Regan,et al.  Some dynamic features of depth perception. , 1973, Vision research.

[9]  D. Regan Visual information channeling in normal and disordered vision. , 1982, Psychological review.

[10]  D. Regan,et al.  Visual field defects for unidirectional and oscillatory motion in depth , 1989, Vision Research.

[11]  D. Regan,et al.  Looming detectors in the human visual pathway , 1978, Vision Research.

[12]  D. Regan,et al.  Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth: Changing-disparity and changing-size feed the same motion-in-depth stage , 1979, Vision Research.

[13]  K. Simons Stereoacuity norms in young children. , 1981, Archives of ophthalmology.

[14]  D Regan,et al.  Visual field defects for vergence eye movements and for stereomotion perception. , 1986, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[15]  G. Gescheider Psychophysics : method, theory, and application , 1985 .

[16]  D Regan,et al.  Visual Judgements and Misjudgements in Cricket, and the Art of Flight , 1992, Perception.

[17]  C Blakemore,et al.  On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  D Regan,et al.  The relation between discrimination and sensitivity in the perception of motion in depth. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  M. Cynader,et al.  Stereoscopic subsystems for position in depth and for motion in depth , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[20]  D Regan,et al.  A stereo field map with implications for disparity processing. , 1973, Investigative ophthalmology.

[21]  D. Regan,et al.  Evidence for the existence of neural mechanisms selectively sensitive to the direction of movement in space , 1973, The Journal of physiology.