Topography of brain electrical activity dissociates the sequential order transformation of verbal versus spatial information in humans

Slow event related potentials (ERPs) of the electroencephalogram were recorded while subjects performed a short-term memory task. They had to mentally transform the order of sequentially presented words (verbal condition) or positions within a grid (spatial condition). The difficulty of the transformation process was varied. The amplitudes of the slow ERPs elicited during the transformation process varied with task difficulty. The topography of this amplitude modulation turned out to be information specific: The maximal effect during transformation of the sequential order of verbal information was found over the left-frontal cortex and during the transformation of spatial information over the parietal cortex. These results suggest that the manipulation of short-term memory contents seems to take place in information specific cortical areas which are thought to be also involved during on-line processing and long-term storage of these informations.

[1]  G. Bower,et al.  Human Associative Memory , 1973 .

[2]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Slow potentials of the cerebral cortex and behavior. , 1990, Physiological reviews.

[4]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Topography of brain electrical activity dissociates the retrieval of spatial versus verbal information from episodic long-term memory in humans , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  H. Huynh,et al.  Conditions under Which Mean Square Ratios in Repeated Measurements Designs Have Exact F-Distributions , 1970 .

[7]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[8]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[9]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  F Rösler,et al.  A correction method for DC drift artifacts. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[11]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET , 1993, Nature.

[12]  A. Damasio Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[14]  M. Verbaten,et al.  Event-Related Brain Research , 1991 .

[15]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Slow negative brain potentials as reflections of specific modular resources of cognition , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[16]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.

[17]  U. Mitzdorf,et al.  Physiological sources of evoked potentials. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[18]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak Functional mapping of verbal memory and language , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  R. Cattell,et al.  Personality, role, mood, and situation-perception: a unifying theory of modulators. , 1963, Psychological review.

[20]  M. Heil,et al.  Distinct Cortical Activation Patterns during Long-Term Memory Retrieval of Verbal, Spatial, and Color Information , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.