Uncertainty is associated with biased expectancies and heightened responses to aversion.

Uncertainty is an omnipresent force in peoples' lives that has been shown to amplify the negative impact of aversive events. This amplified aversiveness, together with the negative attitudes that individuals can have toward uncertainty, suggests that a cue indicating uncertainty about future events might be associated with biased expectancies of negative outcomes or biased contingency estimates, similar to biases that have been observed for traditional fear-relevant cues, such as snakes or spiders. Participants in this study saw three different cues: one that indicated with certainty that an aversive picture would follow, one that indicated with certainty that a neutral picture would follow, and one that indicated uncertainty about whether an aversive or neutral picture would follow. Online self-report data revealed negatively biased expectancies of aversion after uncertain cues. The degree of this online expectancy bias predicted participants' estimates, at the conclusion of the experiment, of the relationship between uncertain cues and aversive pictures. Aversive pictures after the uncertain cue (relative to those after the certain cue) were accompanied by increased skin conductance responses and self-reported negative mood. These findings that uncertainty is accompanied by biased expectancies of aversion and heightened responses to aversion warrant extensions of this research in anxiety disorders, given evidence for intolerance of uncertainty and anticipatory dysfunction in the pathology of such disorders.

[1]  A. Arntz,et al.  Illusory correlation, on-line probability estimates, and electrodermal responding in a (quasi)-conditioning paradigm , 1990, Biological Psychology.

[2]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Carter,et al.  Polishing the windows of the mind. , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[4]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  The feeling of uncertainty intensifies affective reactions. , 2009, Emotion.

[5]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Are covariation biases attributable to a priori expectancy biases? , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[6]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fear-relevant illusory correlations: what types of associations promote judgmental bias? , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[7]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Placebo-Induced Changes in fMRI in the Anticipation and Experience of Pain , 2004, Science.

[8]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Verbal reports as data. , 1980 .

[9]  Hal R. Arkes,et al.  Estimates of contingency between two dichotomous variables. , 1983 .

[10]  H. Critchley,et al.  Fear Conditioning in Humans The Influence of Awareness and Autonomic Arousal on Functional Neuroanatomy , 2002, Neuron.

[11]  K. Mogg,et al.  Fear-relevant selective associations and social anxiety: absence of a positive bias. , 2006, Behaviour research and therapy.

[12]  Peter A Bandettini,et al.  Impact of continuous versus intermittent CS-UCS pairing on human brain activation during Pavlovian fear conditioning. , 2007, Behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  E. Kimmel Judgments of UCS intensity and diminution of the UCR in classical GSR conditioning. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  A. Etkin,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[16]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. , 1969, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[17]  M. Kindt,et al.  Illusory correlation and social anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[18]  Karim Nader,et al.  Ambiguity and anxiety: when a glass half full is empty , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Kindt,et al.  Dissociating response systems: Erasing fear from memory , 2010, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[20]  E. Rolls,et al.  Cognitive Modulation of Olfactory Processing , 2005, Neuron.

[21]  M. Kindt,et al.  Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  W. Grings,et al.  Judgments of UCS intensity and diminution of the unconditioned GSR. , 1971, Psychophysiology.

[23]  P. Pauli,et al.  Covariation Bias in Panic-Prone Individuals , 1996 .

[24]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Placebo Effects Mediated by Endogenous Opioid Activity on μ-Opioid Receptors , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  W. Grings,et al.  Magnitude of UCR as a function of variability in the CS-UCS relationship. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  L. Becerra,et al.  Neural circuitry underlying pain modulation: expectation, hypnosis, placebo , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  M. Freeston,et al.  Generalized anxiety disorder: a preliminary test of a conceptual model. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[28]  Fred J Helmstetter,et al.  Neural Substrates Mediating Human Delay and Trace Fear Conditioning , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Brain mechanisms of expectation associated with insula and amygdala response to aversive taste: Implications for placebo , 2006, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[30]  A. Arntz,et al.  Covariation bias in phobic women: the relationship between a priori expectancy, on-line expectancy, autonomic responding, and a posteriori contingency judgment. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[31]  J. Gross The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review , 1998 .

[32]  P. Pauli,et al.  A priori expectancy bias in patients with panic disorder. , 2001, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[33]  Issidoros C. Sarinopoulos,et al.  Uncertainty during anticipation modulates neural responses to aversion in human insula and amygdala. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  N. Bolger,et al.  Brain Mediators of Predictive Cue Effects on Perceived Pain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  G. Davey,et al.  The expectancy bias model of selective associations: the relationship of judgements of CS dangerousness, CS-UCS similarity and prior fear to a priori and a posteriori covariation assessments. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[37]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Individual differences in some (but not all) medial prefrontal regions reflect cognitive demand while regulating unpleasant emotion , 2009, NeuroImage.

[38]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Predicting events of varying probability: uncertainty investigated by fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[39]  P. Pauli,et al.  Covariation bias in panic-prone individuals. , 1996, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[40]  Tom Johnstone,et al.  Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment response. , 2009, The American journal of psychiatry.

[41]  Stefan Wiens,et al.  Effects of trial order and differential conditioning on acquisition of differential shock expectancy and skin conductance conditioning to masked stimuli. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[42]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[43]  P. Pauli,et al.  A priori expectancy bias and its relation to shock experience and anxiety: a naturalistic study in patients with an automatic implantable cardioverter defibrillator. , 2001, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[44]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  The Devil You Know , 2008, Psychological science.

[45]  M. Dawson,et al.  The electrodermal system , 2007 .

[46]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Altering expectancy dampens neural response to aversive taste in primary taste cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  T. Wager,et al.  Placebo effects in the brain: Linking mental and physiological processes , 2005, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[48]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fear-relevant selective associations and covariation bias. , 1989, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[49]  P. Pauli,et al.  Covariation bias in flight phobics. , 1998, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[50]  T. Borkovec,et al.  The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[51]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of aversion and its anticipation , 2006, NeuroImage.

[52]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[53]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Handbook Of Psychophysiology , 2019 .

[54]  A. Tellegen,et al.  Perception: autonomic response to shock as a function of predictability in time and locus. , 1972, Psychophysiology.

[55]  Dissociations between Covariation Bias and Expectancy Bias for Fear-relevant Stimuli , 1997 .

[56]  P. Boelen,et al.  Intolerance of uncertainty and social anxiety. , 2009, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[57]  David H. Barlow,et al.  Anxiety and its disorders : the nature and treatment of anxiety and panic , 1988 .

[58]  K. Buhr,et al.  The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale: psychometric properties of the English version. , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[59]  H. Merckelbach,et al.  Covariation bias and the return of fear. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[60]  R. Rapee,et al.  Covariation bias for phylogenetic versus ontogenetic fear-relevant stimuli. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[61]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Genesis of popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic observations. , 1967, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[62]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. , 1990, Psychological review.

[63]  E. Kandel,et al.  Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activity in the Amygdala , 2006, Neuron.

[64]  Blood-injection-injury fears: harm- vs. disgust-relevant selective outcome associations. , 2007, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[65]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Functional MRI at the crossroads. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[66]  L. Alloy,et al.  Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information. , 1984 .

[67]  I. Engelhard,et al.  Expectancy bias and the persistence of posttraumatic stress. , 2009, Behaviour research and therapy.

[68]  Katherine E. Prater,et al.  Failure of anterior cingulate activation and connectivity with the amygdala during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder. , 2010, The American journal of psychiatry.

[69]  Peter A. Bandettini,et al.  Neural correlates of unconditioned response diminution during Pavlovian conditioning , 2008, NeuroImage.

[70]  M. Paulus,et al.  An Insular View of Anxiety , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[71]  R. McNally Anxiety and Its Disorders: The Nature and Treatment of Anxiety and Panic, 2nd ed. , 2002 .

[72]  Michael Davis,et al.  The amygdala: vigilance and emotion , 2001, Molecular Psychiatry.

[73]  Nora D. Volkow,et al.  Effects of expectation on the brain metabolic responses to methylphenidate and to its placebo in non-drug abusing subjects , 2006, NeuroImage.

[74]  P. Lang International affective picture system (IAPS) : affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual , 2005 .

[75]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Phylo- and ontogenetic fears and the expectation of danger: differences between spider- and flight-phobic subjects in cognitive and physiological responses to disorder-specific stimuli. , 2006, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[76]  Ben Seymour,et al.  Neural Activity Associated with the Passive Prediction of Ambiguity and Risk for Aversive Events , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[77]  W. Grings,et al.  Effects of trace versus delay conditioning, interstimulus interval variability, and instructions on UCR diminution. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[78]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. , 1999, Science.

[79]  H. Flor,et al.  Covariation bias for ambiguous social stimuli in generalized social phobia. , 2004, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[80]  W. Heller,et al.  Distinguishing neural substrates of heterogeneity among anxiety disorders. , 2005, International review of neurobiology.

[81]  Alan G. Sanfey,et al.  Expectations and social decision-making: biasing effects of prior knowledge on Ultimatum responses , 2009 .

[82]  J. Kagan Motives and development. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[83]  Robert M. Holaway,et al.  A comparison of intolerance of uncertainty in analogue obsessive-compulsive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. , 2006, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[84]  G. Davey An expectancy model of laboratory preparedness effects , 1992 .

[85]  Catherine J. Norris,et al.  Aging is associated with positive responding to neutral information but reduced recovery from negative information. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[86]  D. Shanks,et al.  The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[87]  V. Arolt,et al.  Human Fear Conditioning and Extinction in Neuroimaging: A Systematic Review , 2009, PloS one.

[88]  Oliver J. Hulme,et al.  Modulation of aesthetic value by semantic context: An fMRI study , 2009, NeuroImage.

[89]  M. Craske,et al.  Fear and learning: From basic processes to clinical implications. , 2006 .