Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: a comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian children.

In this study, predictors of reading and spelling difficulties among children learning more transparent (Norwegian/Swedish) and less transparent (English) orthographies were examined longitudinally from preschool through Grade 2 using parallel versions of tests. A series of logistic regression analysis indicated three main findings. First, phonological awareness as a predictor of reading difficulties in the Scandinavian sample was time-limited to Grade 1, but remained as a significant predictor in the English-speaking sample. Second, phonological awareness predicted spelling difficulties similarly across orthographies. Third, preschool and kindergarten RAN was a significant predictor of reading and spelling difficulties at both Grades 1 and 2 across orthographies. The authors conclude that phonological awareness diminishes as a predictor of reading difficulties in transparent orthographies after the first years of schooling, that RAN is a better long-term predictor of reading difficulties, and that phonological awareness is associated with spelling difficulties similarly in transparent and opaque orthographies.

[1]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  M. Caravolas The Nature and Causes of Dyslexia in Different Languages. , 2005 .

[3]  H Lyytinen,et al.  Predicting Delay in Reading Achievement in a Highly Transparent Language , 2001, Journal of learning disabilities.

[4]  Aryan van der Leij,et al.  Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. , 2002 .

[5]  M. Caravolas Spelling Development in Alphabetic Writing Systems: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective , 2004 .

[6]  P. H. Seymour,et al.  Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. , 2003, British journal of psychology.

[7]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. , 1994, Memory.

[8]  V. Muter,et al.  Concurrent and Longitudinal Predictors of Reading: The Role of Metalinguistic and Short‐Term Memory Skills , 1998 .

[9]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system , 1993, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[10]  George K. Georgiou,et al.  Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. , 2008 .

[11]  W. Verhagen,et al.  Phonological Awareness and Naming Speed in the Prediction of Dutch Children's Word Recognition , 2008 .

[12]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. , 1994 .

[13]  M. Snowling,et al.  A Cross-Linguistic Comparison of Children Learning to Read in English and Dutch. , 2004 .

[14]  Richard K. Olson,et al.  Genetic and environmental influences on early literacy , 2006 .

[15]  Ingvar Lundberg,et al.  Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten , 1980 .

[16]  P. Reitsma,et al.  The transient role of explicit phonological recoding for reading acquisition , 2000 .

[17]  Alfred Lie,et al.  Effects of a training program for stimulating skills in word analysis in first-grade children. , 1991 .

[18]  Uta Frith,et al.  Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. , 2003, Child development.

[19]  Charles Hulme,et al.  The Foundations of Spelling Ability: Evidence from a 3-Year Longitudinal Study , 2001 .

[20]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children. , 2000 .

[21]  B. Byrne,et al.  Response to early literacy instruction in the United States, Australia, and Scandinavia: A behavioral-genetic analysis. , 2008, Learning and individual differences.

[22]  C. Hulme,et al.  Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: evidence from Czech and English children. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  B. Byrne,et al.  Longitudinal twin study of early reading development in three countries: Preliminary results , 2002 .

[24]  H. Scarborough,et al.  Predicting, Explaining, and Preventing Children's Reading Difficulties , 2001 .

[25]  V. J. Skjelfjord Phonemic Segmentation. An Important Subskill in Learning to Read: II , 1987 .

[26]  V. Mann,et al.  Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children , 2002 .

[27]  R. Savage,et al.  Rapid Serial Naming Is a Unique Predictor of Spelling in Children , 2008, Journal of learning disabilities.

[28]  Heinz Wimmer,et al.  Dysfluent reading in the absence of spelling difficulties: A specific disability in regular orthographies. , 2002 .

[29]  Brian Byrne,et al.  The Foundation of Literacy: The Child's Acquisition of the Alphabetic Principle , 1998 .

[30]  Patricia Greig Bowers,et al.  Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia , 1993 .

[31]  J. Berko The Child's Learning of English Morphology , 1958 .

[32]  H. Wimmer,et al.  The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. , 2000 .

[33]  Ulla Richardson,et al.  The effects of spelling consistency on phonological awareness: a comparison of English and German. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[34]  K. Müller,et al.  Correlates of early reading performance in a transparent orthography , 2001 .

[35]  H. Wimmer,et al.  The relationship of phonemic awareness to reading acquisition: More consequence than precondition but still important , 1991, Cognition.

[36]  M. Snowling Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia , 1995 .

[37]  P. F. Jong,et al.  Developmental changes in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children learning to read a regular orthography. , 2003 .

[38]  B. Byrne,et al.  Genetic and environmental influences on prereading skills and early reading and spelling development in the United States, Australia, and Scandinavia , 2006 .

[39]  H. Wimmer The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children , 1996 .

[40]  Richard K. Olson,et al.  A behaviour-genetic analysis of orthographic learning, spelling and decoding , 2008 .

[41]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. , 2008 .

[42]  W. Adams,et al.  Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning Second Edition TM Screening Report , 2010 .

[43]  B. Byrne,et al.  Longitudinal twin study of early literacy development: Preschool through Grade 1 , 2006 .

[44]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[45]  Rauno Parrila,et al.  Naming Speed and Phonological Awareness as Predictors of Reading Development. , 2003 .

[46]  M. Wolf,et al.  The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. , 1999 .

[47]  M. Snowling Dyslexia, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[48]  Stephen R. Burgess,et al.  Contributions of Phonological Awareness and Rapid Automatic Naming Ability to the Growth of Word-Reading Skills in Second- to Fifth-Grade Children. , 1997 .

[49]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Components of phonological awareness , 1995 .

[50]  Rollanda E. O'Connor,et al.  Prediction of Reading Disabilities in Kindergarten and First Grade , 1999 .

[51]  B. Pennington,et al.  Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. , 2001, Child development.

[52]  M. Wolf,et al.  Towards a More Universal Understanding of the Developmental Dyslexias: The Contribution of Orthographic Factors , 1994 .

[53]  Kaisa Aunola,et al.  Development of Reading and Spelling Finnish From Preschool to Grade 1 and Grade 2 , 2006 .

[54]  Wimmer and Landerl HOW LEARNING TO SPELL GERMAN DIFFERS FROM LEARNING TO SPELL ENGLISH , 1997 .

[55]  B. Byrne,et al.  Environmental and genetic influences on prereading skills in Australia, Scandinavia, and the United States. , 2005 .

[56]  F. Wood,et al.  Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers , 1998 .

[57]  J. Mitchell Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing , 2001 .

[58]  P. Bryant,et al.  The connection between phonological, syntactic and semantic skills and children’s reading and spelling , 1993 .

[59]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Poor Reading: A Deficit in Skill-Automatization or a Phonological Deficit? , 1998 .

[60]  B. Byrne,et al.  Preliminary results from a longitudinal preschool twin study of early reading development , 2002 .

[61]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Language Basis of Reading and Reading Disabilities: Evidence From a Longitudinal Investigation , 1999 .

[62]  K. Sunseth,et al.  Rapid Naming and Phonemic Awareness: Contributions to Reading, Spelling, and Orthographic Knowledge , 2002 .

[63]  W. Schneider,et al.  Kindergarten letter knowledge, phonological skills, and memory processes: relative effects on early literacy. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[64]  M. Snowling,et al.  The Nonword Reading Deficit in Developmental Dyslexia: A Review. , 1992 .

[65]  P. Zoccolotti,et al.  Do Phonologic and Rapid Automatized Naming Deficits Differentially Affect Dyslexic Children With and Without a History of Language Delay? A Study of Italian Dyslexic Children , 2006, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[66]  P. Reitsma,et al.  Analyzing the development of individual differences in terms of Matthew effects in reading: results from a Dutch Longitudinal study. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[67]  K. P. Bos IQ, phonological awareness and continuous-naming speed related to Dutch poor decoding children's performance on two word identification tests , 1998 .

[68]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Estimating the Risk of Future Reading Difficulties in Kindergarten Children: A Research-Based Model and Its Clinical Implementation. , 2001, Language, speech, and hearing services in schools.

[69]  H. Catts LANGUAGE BASIS OF READING DISABILITIES AND IMPLICATIONS FOR EARLY IDENTIFICATION AND REMEDIATION , 2003 .

[70]  Changing Relations between Phonological Processing Abilities and Word-Level Reading as Children Develop from Beginning to Skilled Readers: A 5-Year Longitudinal Study. , 1997 .

[71]  C. Hulme,et al.  The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: a Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[72]  N. Goulandris Dyslexia in Different Languages: Cross-Linguistic Comparisons , 2003 .

[73]  A. van der Leij,et al.  Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study , 1999 .

[74]  J. L. Fischer Social Influences on the Choice of a Linguistic Variant , 1958 .

[75]  Jack M. Fletcher,et al.  Kindergarten Prediction of Reading Skills: A Longitudinal Comparative Analysis , 2004 .

[76]  B. Byrne,et al.  Longitudinal Twin Study of Early Literacy Development: Preschool and Kindergarten Phases , 2005 .

[77]  Florence Clark,et al.  THE ILLINOIS TEST OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC ABILITIES , 1982 .

[78]  Stefan Samuelsson,et al.  Preschool cognitive and language skills predicting Kindergarten and Grade 1 reading and spelling: a cross-linguistic comparison , 2009 .