Depth perception during saccades.

A number of studies have investigated the localization of briefly flashed targets during saccades to understand how the brain perceptually compensates for changes in gaze direction. Typical version saccades, i.e., saccades between two points of the horopter, are not only associated with changes in gaze direction, but also with large transient changes of ocular vergence. These transient changes in vergence have to be compensated for just as changes in gaze direction. We investigated depth judgments of perisaccadically flashed stimuli relative to continuously present references and report several novel findings. First, disparity thresholds increased around saccade onset. Second, for horizontal saccades, depth judgments were prone to systematic errors: Stimuli flashed around saccade onset were perceived in a closer depth plane than persistently shown references with the same retinal disparity. Briefly before and after this period, flashed stimuli tended to be perceived in a farther depth plane. Third, depth judgments for upward and downward saccades differed substantially: For upward, but not for downward saccades we observed the same pattern of mislocalization as for horizontal saccades. Finally, unlike localization in the fronto-parallel plane, depth judgments did not critically depend on the presence of visual references. Current models fail to account for the observed pattern of mislocalization in depth.

[1]  Eric Castet,et al.  Dynamics of attentional deployment during saccadic programming. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[2]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[3]  Jordan Pola,et al.  Models of the mechanism underlying perceived location of a perisaccadic flash , 2004, Vision Research.

[4]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Perceived geometrical relationships affected by eye-movement signals , 1997, Nature.

[5]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  H. Honda The time courses of visual mislocalization and of extraretinal eye position signals at the time of vertical saccades , 1991, Vision Research.

[8]  Anna Ma-Wyatt,et al.  Seeing and ballistic pointing at perisaccadic targets. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[9]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[10]  A. Parker,et al.  Neuronal Computation of Disparity in V1 Limits Temporal Resolution for Detecting Disparity Modulation , 2005, Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Lappe,et al.  Contrast dependency of saccadic compression and suppression , 2004, Vision Research.

[12]  M. Morrone,et al.  Extraretinal Control of Saccadic Suppression , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human vertical saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  M Concetta Morrone,et al.  Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Senders,et al.  Eye Movements and Psychological Processes , 1976 .

[16]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Dynamic allocation of visual attention during the execution of sequences of saccades , 2004, Vision Research.