Social Rejection but Not Ostracism Increases Cognitive Effort Avoidance

Purpose Social exclusion has been found to have a significant impact on cognitive control processing. However, the existing research on this topic has yielded inconsistent findings, possibly due to variations in the type of exclusion and individuals’ cognitive effort. Two studies were conducted to explore the influence of social rejection and ostracism on cognitive effort avoidance. Participants and Methods Study 1 involved forty-six adults who were randomly divided into a rejection group and a control group using a get-acquainted paradigm. The demand selection task (DST) was used to measure cognitive effort avoidance. In Study 2, forty-eight adults were recruited, Cyberball and DST paradigms were used to evoke ostracism and test cognitive effort avoidance, respectively. Results The results of study 1 showed that individuals who were socially rejected by their partners exhibited impaired response accuracy of cognitive control and increased cognitive effort avoidance. This indicates that social rejection has a negative impact on cognitive control processing and that individuals may be more likely to avoid cognitive effort when experiencing social rejection. The results of study 2 showed that ostracism had an impact on both response speed and accuracy, but it did not significantly affect cognitive effort avoidance. This indicates that social rejection affects cognitive control processing differently than ostracism, and individuals are more likely to avoid cognitive effort when experiencing social rejection. Conclusion These findings suggest that social rejection and ostracism have different effects on cognitive effort, which may contribute to the inconsistent cognitive performance during social exclusion. Future research may explore the underlying mechanisms that lead to these differences and examine how individuals can mitigate the negative effects of social exclusion on cognitive control processing.

[1]  K. Williams,et al.  Exploring social exclusion: Where we are and where We're going , 2022, Social and Personality Psychology Compass.

[2]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  The impact of punishment on cognitive control in a clinical population characterized by heightened punishment sensitivity. , 2021, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[3]  A. Bertrams,et al.  Measuring Motivation for Cognitive Effort as State , 2021, Frontiers in Psychology.

[4]  A. R. Otto,et al.  Acute Psychosocial Stress Increases Cognitive-Effort Avoidance , 2021, Psychological science.

[5]  Kimberly S. Chiew,et al.  Revisiting positive affect and reward influences on cognitive control , 2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[6]  O. Wolf,et al.  Delayed effects of acute stress on cognitive emotion regulation , 2020, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[7]  H. Bäck,et al.  Exploring a pathway to radicalization: The effects of social exclusion and rejection sensitivity , 2020, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations.

[8]  Debbie M. Yee,et al.  Dissociable influences of reward and punishment on adaptive cognitive control , 2020, bioRxiv.

[9]  A. R. Otto,et al.  It's all relative: Reward-induced cognitive control modulation depends on context. , 2020, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Senqing Qi,et al.  Social exclusion modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs , 2020, Human brain mapping.

[11]  Y. Paloyelis,et al.  Mapping social reward and punishment processing in the human brain: A voxel-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging findings using the social incentive delay task , 2020, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  When effort matters: Expectations of reward and efficacy guide cognitive control allocation , 2020, bioRxiv.

[13]  L. Fournier,et al.  Free-choice and forced-choice actions: Shared representations and conservation of cognitive effort , 2020, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[14]  Daniel V. Holt,et al.  A cognitive control framework for understanding emotion regulation flexibility. , 2020, Emotion.

[15]  M. Marinucci,et al.  How intergroup social connections shape immigrants’ responses to social exclusion , 2020 .

[16]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Social exclusion affects working memory performance in young adolescent girls , 2019, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Jessica I. Määttä,et al.  Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work , 2019, Science.

[18]  G. Gendolla,et al.  Affect and cognitive control: Insights from research on effort mobilization. , 2019, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[19]  Sukki Yoon,et al.  Social exclusion influences on the effectiveness of altruistic versus egoistic appeals in charitable advertising , 2019, Marketing Letters.

[20]  Christopher Y. Olivola,et al.  The Effort Paradox: Effort Is Both Costly and Valued , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  A. R. Otto,et al.  Cognitive capacity limitations and Need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure , 2018, Cognition.

[22]  Christoph Braun,et al.  How the brain reacts to social stress (exclusion) – A scoping review , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Toward a Rational and Mechanistic Account of Mental Effort. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  Regan L. Mandryk,et al.  The Effects of Social Exclusion on Play Experience and Hostile Cognitions in Digital Games , 2016, CHI.

[25]  J. Belsky,et al.  Social exclusion predicts impaired self-regulation: a 2-year longitudinal panel study including the transition from preschool to school. , 2015, Journal of personality.

[26]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Emotional foundations of cognitive control , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  M. Berger,et al.  “More than skin deep”: stress neurobiology and mental health consequences of racial discrimination , 2015, Stress.

[28]  K. Gonsalkorale,et al.  Are all ostracism experiences equal? A comparison of the autobiographical recall, Cyberball, and O-Cam paradigms , 2014, Behavior research methods.

[29]  H. Nittono,et al.  Cognitive, Affective, and Motivational Changes during Ostracism: An ERP, EMG, and EEG Study Using a Computerized Cyberball Task , 2013, Neuroscience journal.

[30]  K. Jonas,et al.  Out of the group, out of control? The brain responds to social exclusion with changes in cognitive control. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[31]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Expected Value of Control: An Integrative Theory of Anterior Cingulate Cortex Function , 2013, Neuron.

[32]  Joris Lammers,et al.  Not all rejections are alike; competence and warmth as a fundamental distinction in social rejection , 2013 .

[33]  S. Dys,et al.  Adolescents' emotions and reasoning in contexts of moral conflict and social exclusion. , 2012, New directions for youth development.

[34]  Naomi I. Eisenberger,et al.  The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  Sara C. Verosky,et al.  Individual differences in neural responses to social rejection: the joint effect of self-esteem and attentional control. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[36]  Hiroshi Nittono,et al.  Does low self-esteem enhance social pain? The relationship between trait self-esteem and anterior cingulate cortex activation induced by ostracism. , 2010, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[37]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[38]  T. Robbins,et al.  Serotoninergic regulation of emotional and behavioural control processes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  Roz Dixon,et al.  Ostracism , 2007 .

[40]  Julie J. Exline,et al.  Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  J. Pateman,et al.  SOCIAL EXCLUSION , 1997, Journal of Social Policy.

[45]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  Guangming Ran,et al.  Do attachment patterns predict aggression in a context of social rejection? An executive functioning account , 2017, Aggressive behavior.

[47]  P. Riva,et al.  Social exclusion : psychological approaches to understanding and reducing its impact , 2016 .

[48]  Roy F. Baumeister,et al.  Social exclusion causes a shift toward prevention motivation , 2015 .

[49]  T. Heatherton Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[50]  R. Baumeister,et al.  If you can't join them, beat them: effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.