Which cognitive functions subserve clustering and switching in category fluency? Generalisations from an extended set of semantic categories using linear mixed-effects modelling

Clustering and switching are hypothesised to reflect the automatic and controlled components in category fluency, respectively, but how they are associated with cognitive functions has not been fully elucidated, due to several uncertainties. (1) The conventional scoring method that segregates responses by semantic categories could not optimally dissociate the automatic and controlled components. (2) The temporal structure of individual responses, as characterised by mean retrieval time (MRT) and mean switching time (MST), has seldom been analysed alongside the more well-studied variables, cluster size (CS) and number of switches (NS). (3) Most studies examined only one to a few semantic categories, raising concerns of generalisability. This study built upon a distance-based automatic clustering procedure, referred to as temporal–semantic distance procedure, to thoroughly characterise the category fluency performance. Linear mixed-effects (LME) modelling was applied to re-examine the differential associations of clustering and switching with cognitive functions with a sample of 80 university students. Our results revealed that although lexical retrieval speed (LRS) is clearly the determining factor for effective clustering and switching, matrix reasoning and processing speed also have significant roles to play, possibly in the processes of identifying and validating the semantic relationships. Interestingly, total fluency score was accurately predicted by the four clustering/switching indices alone; including the cognitive variables did not significantly improve the prediction. These findings underline the importance of the clustering and switching indices in explaining the category fluency performance and the cognitive demands in category fluency.

[1]  C. Adcock,et al.  Primary Mental Abilities. , 1971, The Journal of general psychology.

[2]  J. Gordon,et al.  Why do older adults have difficulty with semantic fluency? , 2018, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[3]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[4]  Federica Scarpina,et al.  The Stroop Color and Word Test , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[5]  R. Engle,et al.  The role of working memory capacity in retrieval. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  A. Berger,et al.  Analysis of word clustering in verbal fluency of school-aged children. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[7]  Stephen Camarata,et al.  Sex differences in processing speed: Developmental effects in males and females , 2006 .

[8]  P. Feyereisen,et al.  Verbal Knowledge as a Compensation Determinant of Adult Age Differences in Verbal Fluency Tasks over Time , 2011 .

[9]  D. Altman,et al.  Statistics notes: Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: Part 1—correlation within subjects , 1995 .

[10]  W. Rosen,et al.  Verbal fluency in aging and dementia , 1980 .

[11]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[12]  A. Meyer,et al.  What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[13]  Arthur L. Benton,et al.  Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease , 1968 .

[14]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Fluid Reasoning and the Developing Brain , 2009, Frontiers in neuroscience.

[15]  S. Kiran,et al.  Semantic fluency in aphasia: clustering and switching in the course of 1 minute. , 2017, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[16]  D. G. Clark,et al.  Latent information in fluency lists predicts functional decline in persons at risk for Alzheimer disease , 2014, Cortex.

[17]  R. Engle,et al.  Is working memory capacity task dependent , 1989 .

[18]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[19]  D. Bates,et al.  Balancing Type I Error and Power in Linear Mixed Models , 2015, 1511.01864.

[20]  P. Ellen Grant,et al.  Developmental neural networks in children performing a Categorical N-Back Task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[21]  Ulrich H. Frauenfelder,et al.  On the effects of L2 perception and of individual differences in L1 production on L2 pronunciation , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[22]  Bodo Winter,et al.  Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications , 2013, ArXiv.

[23]  G. Band,et al.  Online games training aging brains: limited transfer to cognitive control functions , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[24]  R. Cabeza Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[25]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[26]  Andrzej T. Galecki,et al.  Linear mixed-effects models using R , 2013 .

[27]  J. Bryan,et al.  Adult Age Differences in Strategy Use During Verbal Fluency Performance , 2002, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[28]  Gene A. Brewer,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Variation in Verbal Fluency: a Latent Variable Analysis of Clustering, Switching, and Overall Performance , 2022 .

[29]  Barry Gordon,et al.  Capturing additional information about the organization of entries in the lexicon from verbal fluency productions , 2014, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[30]  R. Mahurin,et al.  WAIS-III Matrix Reasoning test performance in a mixed clinical sample. , 1999, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[31]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Timed picture naming in seven languages , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[33]  D. Bjorklund,et al.  Associative and categorical processes in children's memory: The role of automaticity in the development of organization in free recall ☆ , 1985 .

[34]  M. Crossley,et al.  Clustering and Switching Strategies During Verbal Fluency Performance Differentiate Alzheimer's Disease and Healthy Aging , 2011, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[35]  J. Crawford,et al.  Verbal knowledge and speed of information processing as mediators of age differences in verbal fluency performance among older adults. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[36]  P. Hoffman Divergent effects of healthy ageing on semantic knowledge and control: Evidence from novel comparisons with semantically impaired patients , 2018, bioRxiv.

[37]  E. Gleichgerrcht,et al.  The relationship between executive functions and fluid intelligence in Parkinson's disease , 2012, Psychological Medicine.

[38]  Q. Guo,et al.  Clustering and switching during a semantic verbal fluency test contribute to differential diagnosis of cognitive impairment , 2013, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[39]  M. Linden,et al.  Use of the Hayling task to measure inhibition of prepotent responses in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[40]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[41]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations , 2010, NeuroImage.

[42]  D. Abwender,et al.  Qualitative Analysis of Verbal Fluency Output: Review and Comparison of Several Scoring Methods , 2001, Assessment.

[43]  Trey Hedden,et al.  Contributions of Processing Ability and Knowledge to Verbal Memory Tasks across the Adult Life-Span , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[44]  M. Crossley,et al.  The effect of age and sex on clustering and switching during speeded verbal fluency tasks , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[45]  D. Donaldson,et al.  Association and not semantic relationships elicit the N400 effect: electrophysiological evidence from an explicit language comprehension task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[46]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. , 1991 .

[47]  V. Carey,et al.  Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-Plus , 2001 .

[48]  Brodie Hughes,et al.  MISSILE WOUNDS OF THE BRAIN A Study of Psychological Deficits , 1970 .

[49]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[50]  J. Wixted,et al.  Analyzing the dynamics of free recall: An integrative review of the empirical literature , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[51]  Jing Cheng,et al.  Real longitudinal data analysis for real people: Building a good enough mixed model , 2010, Statistics in medicine.

[52]  Tony J Prescott,et al.  A new dissimilarity measure for finding semantic structure in category fluency data with implications for understanding memory organization in schizophrenia. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[53]  André W. Keizer,et al.  The effect of gamma enhancing neurofeedback on the control of feature bindings and intelligence measures. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[54]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[55]  W. A. Bousfield,et al.  An Analysis of Sequences of Restricted Associative Responses , 1944 .

[56]  G. Winocur,et al.  Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[57]  A. Troyer Normative Data for Clustering and Switching on Verbal Fluency Tasks , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[58]  J. Bakdash,et al.  Repeated Measures Correlation , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[59]  T. Sunderland,et al.  Apolipoprotein E and category fluency: evidence for reduced semantic access in healthy normal controls at risk for developing Alzheimer's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  Tilo Kircher,et al.  Automatic processing of semantic relations in fMRI: Neural activation during semantic priming of taxonomic and thematic categories , 2008, Brain Research.

[61]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  On the dissociation between clustering and switching in verbal fluency: comment on Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander and Stuss , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[63]  Tomasz Burzykowski,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using R: A Step-by-Step Approach , 2013 .

[64]  G. Lockhead,et al.  The free recall of category examples. , 1980 .

[65]  D. Altman,et al.  Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: Part 2—correlation between subjects , 1995, BMJ.

[66]  Arthur L. Benton,et al.  Word fluency and brain damage , 1967 .

[67]  Tamiko Azuma,et al.  Working memory and perseveration in verbal fluency. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[68]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.