The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Garrett,et al. Grammatical Gender Is on the Tip of Italian Tongues , 1997 .
[2] E. Kaplan,et al. The “Tip-of-the-Tongue” Phenomenon in Aphasia , 1976, Cortex.
[3] José E. García-Albea,et al. On the autonomy of phonological encoding: Evidence from slips of the tongue in Spanish , 1991 .
[4] A. Caramazza,et al. Dissociation of inflectional and derivational morphology , 1988, Brain and Language.
[5] Claus-W. Wallesch,et al. Errors and their Relevance for Models of Language Production , 1993 .
[6] Zenzi M. Griffin,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article WHAT THE EYES SAY ABOUT SPEAKING , 2022 .
[7] W. Levelt,et al. Monitoring the Time Course of Phonological Encoding , 1995 .
[8] A. Cutler. Slips of the Tongue and Language Production , 1982 .
[9] M. Miozzo,et al. The two-stage model of lexical retrieval: evidence from a case of anomia with selective preservation of grammatical gender , 1995, Cognition.
[10] M. Pickering,et al. The Representation of Verbs: Evidence from Syntactic Priming in Language Production , 1998 .
[11] A. Postma. Detection of errors during speech production: a review of speech monitoring models , 2000, Cognition.
[12] R. Hartsuiker,et al. Syntactic Persistence in Dutch , 1998, Language and speech.
[13] N. Martin,et al. Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: Implications for models of lexical retrieval☆ , 1989 .
[14] Harry A. Whitaker,et al. Studies in neurolinguistics , 1976 .
[15] Gerard Kempen,et al. The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: indirect election of words , 1983, Cognition.
[16] W. Glaser,et al. The time course of picture-word interference. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Agrammatic Production of Subject–Verb Agreement: The Effect of Conceptual Number , 1999, Brain and Language.
[18] Kathryn Bock,et al. Meaning, sound, and syntax: Lexical priming in sentence production. , 1986 .
[19] F. Michèl,et al. Lexicalization in an anomic patient , 1989, Neuropsychologia.
[20] H. Kolk,et al. The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. , 1993, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[21] Kathleen M. Eberhard,et al. The Accessibility of Conceptual Number to the Processes of Subject–Verb Agreement in English☆☆☆ , 1999 .
[22] Julie Franck,et al. The role of syntactic tree structure and complexity in subject-verb agreement , 2002 .
[23] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .
[24] George A. Miller,et al. Language and Communication , 1951 .
[25] R. Hartsuiker,et al. Syntactic Facilitation in Agrammatic Sentence Production , 1998, Brain and Language.
[26] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: II. Feedback, expectation, olfaction, illusions , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.
[27] R. Jakobson. Boas' View of Grammatical Meaning , 1959 .
[28] Brian Butterworth,et al. Subject-verb agreement in Spanish and English: Differences in the role of conceptual constraints , 1996, Cognition.
[29] A. Newell. You can't play 20 questions with nature and win : projective comments on the papers of this symposium , 1973 .
[30] D. G. MacKay. Awareness and error detection: New theories and research paradigms , 1992, Consciousness and Cognition.
[31] W. Levelt,et al. Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form , 1994 .
[32] G. Kempen,et al. Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: a computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar , 2000, Cognition.
[33] A. W. Ellis. Normality and pathology in cognitive functions , 1982 .
[34] L. Finkel,et al. Integration of distributed cortical systems by reentry: a computer simulation of interactive functionally segregated visual areas , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[35] D. Caplan,et al. Biological studies of mental processes , 1981 .
[36] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Error Monitoring in Speech Production: A Computational Test of the Perceptual Loop Theory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[37] Joseph P. Levy,et al. Connectionist models of memory and language , 1995 .
[38] Roger S. Brown,et al. The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .
[39] W. Lambert,et al. The French Speaker's Skill with Grammatical Gender: An Example of Rule-Governed Behavior , 1977 .
[40] Gerard Kempen,et al. An Incremental Procedural Grammar for Sentence Formulation , 1987, Cogn. Sci..
[41] Alan Garnham,et al. Slips of the tongue in the London-Lund corpus of spontaneous conversation , 1981 .
[42] Eric Wanner,et al. Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .
[43] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[44] J. Nicol,et al. Separating hierarchical relations and word order in language production: is proximity concord syntactic or linear? , 1998, Cognition.
[45] M. Garrett,et al. When reading a sentence is easier than reading a ‘little’ word: The role of production processes in deep dyslexics' reading aloud , 1998 .
[46] Gary S. Dell,et al. Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data , 1981 .
[47] Brian MacWhinney,et al. Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study , 1982, Cognition.
[48] Mark Steedman,et al. Interaction with context during human sentence processing , 1988, Cognition.
[49] Ulrich Schade,et al. Repairs in a Connectionist Language-Production Model , 1993 .
[50] G. Dell,et al. Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.
[51] Kathryn Bock,et al. Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .
[52] Fernanda Ferreira. Creation of prosody during sentence production. , 1993 .
[53] R. Hartsuiker,et al. Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production , 2000, Cognition.
[54] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995) , 1996 .
[55] Anne Cutler,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[56] Victor S. Ferreira,et al. Syntactic Influences on Lexical and Morphological Processing in Language Production , 2001 .
[57] T. Vance,et al. Japanese/Korean linguistics , 1999 .
[58] G. Dell,et al. Models of Impaired Lexical Access in Speech Production , 2000 .
[59] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the `tip-of-the-tongue' phenomenon , 1997, Cognition.
[60] Zenzi M. Griffin,et al. Constraint, Word Frequency, and the Relationship between Lexical Processing Levels in Spoken Word Production , 1998 .
[61] Brian Butterworth,et al. Speech and talk , 1980 .
[62] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing , 1999, Cogn. Sci..
[63] J. K. Bock,et al. Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation , 1985, Cognition.
[64] Kathryn Bock,et al. Meaning, sound and syntax in english number agreement , 1993 .
[65] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[66] D Norris,et al. Merging information in speech recognition: Feedback is never necessary , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[67] Kathryn Bock,et al. Toward a Cognitive Psychology of Syntax: Information Processing Contributions to Sentence Formulation , 1982 .
[68] Trevor A. Harley,et al. What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighbourhood effects in speech production , 1998 .
[69] M. H. Kelly,et al. Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. , 1992, Psychological review.
[70] G. Vigliocco,et al. A semantic analysis of grammatical class impairments: semantic representations of object nouns, action nouns and action verbs , 2002, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[71] S. Levinson,et al. Rethinking Linguistic Relativity , 1991, Current Anthropology.
[72] A Caramazza,et al. Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-the-tongue states. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[73] Trevor A. Harley,et al. Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .
[74] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[75] M. Garrett. Levels of processing in sentence production , 1980 .
[76] G. Dell,et al. Language production and serial order: a functional analysis and a model. , 1997, Psychological review.
[77] J. K. Bock. Syntactic persistence in language production , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.
[78] Hugh W. Buckingham,et al. 7 – Linguistic Aspects of Lexical Retrieval Disturbances in the Posterior Fluent Aphasias , 1979 .
[79] Alan S. Brown,et al. A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[80] J. Laver,et al. Slips of the tongue. , 1968, The British journal of disorders of communication.
[81] K. Bock,et al. From conceptual roles to structural relations: bridging the syntactic cleft. , 1992 .
[82] P. Starreveld,et al. What about phonological facilitation, response-set membership, and phonological coactivation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[83] Kathryn Bock,et al. A sketchbook of production problems , 1991 .
[84] J. Fodor. Précis of The Modularity of Mind , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[85] David Howard,et al. On the Origin of Semantic Errors in Naming: Evidence from the Case of a Global Aphasic , 1984 .
[86] Victoria A. Fromkin,et al. The Non-Anomalous Nature of Anomalous Utterances , 1971 .
[87] Gabriella Vigliocco,et al. Adjectives and Adjectival Nouns in Japanese: Psychological Processes in Sentence Production. , 1998 .
[88] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Object Attraction in Subject-Verb Agreement Construction , 2001 .
[89] W. Levelt,et al. Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[90] Mark Steedman,et al. On not being led up the garden path : The use of context by the psychological syntax processor , 1985 .
[91] S. Grossberg,et al. Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: I. Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.
[92] A. Caramazza. How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .
[93] K Nakayama,et al. Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. , 1992, Science.
[94] B. MacWhinney,et al. Functionalism and the competition model , 1989 .
[95] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. Crossing the Boundaries in Linguistics , 1981 .
[96] A. Lecours,et al. Biological perspectives on language , 1984 .
[97] Susan M. Garnsey,et al. Semantic Influences On Parsing: Use of Thematic Role Information in Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution , 1994 .
[98] Gabriella Vigliocco,et al. Contact points between lexical retrieval and sentence production , 1999 .
[99] G Vigliocco,et al. Syntactic Accuracy in Sentence Production: The Case of Gender Disagreement in Italian Language-Impaired and Unimpaired Speakers , 1999, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[100] Edeltrud Marx,et al. Gender Processing in Speech Production: Evidence from German Speech Errors , 1999 .
[101] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. Lexical search and order of mention in sentence production , 1981 .
[102] Thomas G. Bever,et al. The Emperor's Psycholinguistics , 1998 .
[103] Gerard Kempen,et al. Incremental syntactic tree formation in human sentence processing: A cognitive architecture based on activation decay and simulated annealing , 1989 .
[104] B. Rapp,et al. Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. , 2000, Psychological review.
[105] Robert J Hartsuiker,et al. Morphophonological influences on the construction of subject – verb agreement , 2004 .
[106] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Perceptual differentiation as a source of category effects in object processing: Evidence from naming and object decision , 1997, Memory & cognition.
[107] D. Slobin. The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition , 1987 .
[108] V. Ferreira. Is It Better to Give Than to Donate? Syntactic Flexibility in Language Production , 1996 .
[109] D. Slobin. From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .
[110] S. Grossberg. The complementary brain: unifying brain dynamics and modularity , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[111] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution , 1994 .
[112] Merrill F. Garrett,et al. Remarks on the Architecture of Language Processing Systems , 2000 .
[113] Elizabeth Bates,et al. Gender and lexical access in Italian , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[114] A. Ellis,et al. A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia. Implications for psychological models of word retrieval. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[115] G. Vigliocco,et al. When Sex and Syntax Go Hand in Hand: Gender Agreement in Language Production , 1999 .
[116] G. Vigliocco,et al. Can Independence Be Observed in a Dependent System? The Case of Tip-of-the-Tongue States , 1999, Brain and Language.
[117] Antje S. Meyer,et al. The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .
[118] J. Cutting,et al. Minimodularity and the perception of layout. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[119] M. F. Garrett,et al. The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .
[120] P. Johnson-Laird. Mental models , 1989 .
[121] B. Baars. Experimental slips and human error : exploring the architecture of volition , 1992 .
[122] S. Grossberg. The Complementary Brain: A Unifying View of Brain Specialization and Modularity , 1998 .
[123] Anne Cutler,et al. Sharpening Ockham's razor , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[124] Aleksandr R. Luria,et al. Language and brain , 1974 .
[125] William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Lexical Representation and Process , 1991 .
[126] David P. Vinson,et al. Is "Count" and "Mass" Information Available When the Noun Is Not? An Investigation of Tip of the Tongue States and Anomia , 1999 .
[127] Thomas Berg,et al. The aftermath of error occurrence: Psycholinguistic evidence from cut-offs , 1986 .
[128] K. Bock,et al. The Ties That Bind: Creating Number Agreement in Speech , 1999 .
[129] Joseph Paul Stemberger,et al. The lexicon in a model of language production , 1985 .
[130] P. Starreveld,et al. Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. , 1996 .
[131] Julie E. Boland,et al. Interaction with autonomy: Multiple Output models and the inadequacy of the Great Divide , 1996, Cognition.
[132] Nadine Martin,et al. Phonological Facilitation of Semantic Errors in Normal and Aphasic Speakers , 1996 .
[133] K. Bock,et al. Some Attractions of Verb Agreement , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[134] V. Ferreira,et al. Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[135] A. Ellis. Progress in the psychology of language , 1985 .
[136] G. Dell,et al. Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: a study of the role of experience in language production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[137] R. R. Peterson,et al. Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .
[138] Brian Butterworth,et al. Constructing Subject-Verb Agreement in Speech: The Role of Semantic and Morphological Factors , 1995 .
[139] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Priming Word Order in Sentence Production , 1999 .
[140] Gary S. Dell,et al. Effects of Frequency and Vocabulary Type on Phonological Speech Errors , 1990 .
[141] K. Bock. Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production , 1992 .
[142] W. Chase,et al. Visual information processing. , 1974 .
[143] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[144] J. Jescheniak,et al. Discrete serial versus cascaded processing in lexical access in speech production : Further evidence from the coactivation of near-synonyms , 1998 .
[145] A. Cutler. The reliability of speech error data , 1981 .
[146] M. Landy,et al. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.
[147] Compositional semantics and the lemma dilemma , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[148] Janet L. McDonald,et al. Properties of Phonological Markers That Affect the Acquisition of Gender-Like Subclasses☆☆☆★ , 1998 .
[149] Brian Butterworth,et al. Disorders of phonological encoding , 1992, Cognition.
[150] K Bock,et al. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? , 1992, Memory & cognition.
[151] Manuel Carreiras,et al. When Words Have Two Genders: Anaphor Resolution for Italian Functionally Ambiguous Words , 1997 .
[152] S. Engel. Thought and Language , 1964 .
[153] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. One or more labels on the bottles? Notional concord in Dutch and French , 1996 .
[154] D. Schacter,et al. On the Tip of the Tongue An Event-Related fMRI Study of Semantic Retrieval Failure and Cognitive Conflict , 2001, Neuron.
[155] G S Dell,et al. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.
[156] Markus F. Damian,et al. Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. , 1999 .
[157] D. G. MacKay,et al. Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue , 1975 .
[158] Gabriella Vigliocco,et al. When Sex Affects Syntax: Contextual Influences in Sentence Production , 2001 .
[159] Trevor A. Harley,et al. A Critique of Top-down Independent Levels Models of Speech Production: Evidence from Non-plan-Internal Speech Errors , 1984, Cogn. Sci..
[160] Bernard J. Baars,et al. Error-Minimizing Mechanisms Boosting or Editing? , 1992 .
[161] W. Levelt,et al. Monitoring and self-repair in speech , 1983, Cognition.
[162] K. Bock. An Effect of the Accessibility of Word Forms on Sentence Structures , 1987 .
[163] G. Kempen,et al. A dual system for producing self-repairs in spontaneous speech: Evidence from experimentally elicited corrections , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[164] Michael K. Tanenhaus,et al. Dynamical models of sentence processing , 1999, Cogn. Sci..
[165] J. Alonso,et al. Influence of layer V of area 18 of the cat visual cortex on responses of cells in layer V of area 17 to stimuli of high velocity , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[166] B. MacWhinney,et al. The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. , 1992 .
[167] Patricia J. Brooks,et al. Acquisition of Gender-like Noun Subclasses in an Artificial Language: The Contribution of Phonological Markers to Learning , 1993 .
[168] V. Fromkin. Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue , 1982 .
[169] Wido La Heij,et al. Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. , 1995 .