Visual Processing of Contour Patterns under Conditions of Inattentional Blindness

An inattentional blindness paradigm was adapted to measure ERPs elicited by visual contour patterns that were or were not consciously perceived. In the first phase of the experiment, subjects performed an attentionally demanding task while task-irrelevant line segments formed square-shaped patterns or random configurations. After the square patterns had been presented 240 times, subjects' awareness of these patterns was assessed. More than half of all subjects, when queried, failed to notice the square patterns and were thus considered inattentionally blind during this first phase. In the second phase of the experiment, the task and stimuli were the same, but following this phase, all of the subjects reported having seen the patterns. ERPs recorded over the occipital pole differed in amplitude from 220 to 260 msec for the pattern stimuli compared with the random arrays regardless of whether subjects were aware of the patterns. At subsequent latencies (300–340 msec) however, ERPs over bilateral occipital-parietal areas differed between patterns and random arrays only when subjects were aware of the patterns. Finally, in a third phase of the experiment, subjects viewed the same stimuli, but the task was altered so that the patterns became task relevant. Here, the same two difference components were evident but were followed by a series of additional components that were absent in the first two phases of the experiment. We hypothesize that the ERP difference at 220–260 msec reflects neural activity associated with automatic contour integration whereas the difference at 300–340 msec reflects visual awareness, both of which are dissociable from task-related postperceptual processing.

[1]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Wu Li,et al.  Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  The spatiotemporal profile of cortical processing leading up to visual perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[4]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Interactions between attention, context and learning in primary visual cortex , 2000, Vision Research.

[5]  P. Bressan,et al.  Auditory Attention Causes Visual Inattentional Blindness , 2008, Perception.

[6]  Su-Ling Yeh,et al.  Dissociation of processing time and awareness by the inattentional blindness paradigm , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[7]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neuroimaging of visual awareness in patients and normal subjects , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[8]  Manfred Fahle,et al.  The electrophysiological correlate of contour integration is similar for color and luminance mechanisms. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[9]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Kanizsa subjective figures capture visual spatial attention: evidence from electrophysiological and behavioral data , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[11]  M. Koivisto,et al.  Independence of visual awareness from attention at early processing stages , 2005, Neuroreport.

[12]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Toward a computational theory of conscious processing , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  Roman Bauer,et al.  Contour integration in striate cortex. Classic cell responses or cooperative selection? , 2002, Experimental brain research.

[14]  C. Koch,et al.  Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  N. Block Two neural correlates of consciousness , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  The influence of inattention on the neural correlates of scene segmentation , 2006, Brain Research.

[17]  V. Lamme Towards a true neural stance on consciousness , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  I. Rock,et al.  Inattentional blindness: Perception without attention. , 1998 .

[19]  Sirko Straube,et al.  The electrophysiological correlate of saliency: Evidence from a figure-detection task , 2010, Brain Research.

[20]  Su-Ling Yeh,et al.  New objects do not capture attention without a top-down setting: Evidence from an inattentional blindness task , 2007 .

[21]  J. Changeux,et al.  Neural Mechanisms for Access to Consciousness , 2004 .

[22]  Michael A. Pitts,et al.  Early Stages of Figure–Ground Segregation during Perception of the Face–Vase , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Michael Bach,et al.  Attention and visual texture segregation. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[24]  R. Hari,et al.  Visual awareness of objects correlates with activity of right occipital cortex , 1996, Neuroreport.

[25]  U. Neisser,et al.  Selective looking: Attending to visually specified events , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Objects Are Highlighted by Spatial Attention , 2006 .

[27]  Carsten Nicolas Boehler,et al.  Binding 3-D Object Perception in the Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  The Role of Spatial Attention in the Selection of Real and Illusory Objects , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Field,et al.  Integration of contours: new insights , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Mika Koivisto,et al.  Event-related brain potential correlates of visual awareness , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[31]  John-Dylan Haynes,et al.  Decoding visual consciousness from human brain signals , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Michael Bach,et al.  Summation of texture segregation across orientation and spatial frequency: electrophysiological and psychophysical findings , 2000, Vision Research.

[33]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Brain Dynamics Underlying the Nonlinear Threshold for Access to Consciousness , 2007, PLoS biology.

[35]  Paola Bressan,et al.  The attentional cost of inattentional blindness , 2008, Cognition.

[36]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Visual Perceptual Learning in Human Object Recognition Areas: A Repetition Priming Study Using High-Density Electrical Mapping , 2001, NeuroImage.

[37]  Delphine Pins,et al.  The neural correlates of conscious vision. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  H. Egeth,et al.  Perception without attention: evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. , 1997, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[39]  Daryl Fougnie,et al.  Executive working memory load induces inattentional blindness , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  Asaid Khateb,et al.  Neural processing of illusory and real contours revealed by high‐density ERP mapping , 2002, Neuroreport.

[41]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Perceptual learning and top-down influences in primary visual cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys , 1995, Neuron.

[43]  C. Chabris,et al.  Gorillas in Our Midst: Sustained Inattentional Blindness for Dynamic Events , 1999, Perception.

[44]  W. Singer,et al.  Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Asaid Khateb,et al.  Visual recognition of faces, objects, and words using degraded stimuli: Where and when it occurs , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[46]  W. Singer,et al.  Neuroelectromagnetic Correlates of Perceptual Closure Processes , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  G. Campana,et al.  Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence of independent facilitation by collinearity and similarity in texture grouping and segmentation , 2009, Vision Research.

[48]  Mika Koivisto,et al.  The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[49]  I. Rock,et al.  Perceptual organization and attention , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al.  Feedforward and Recurrent Processing in Scene Segmentation: Electroencephalography and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Jean Bennett,et al.  Lateral Connectivity and Contextual Interactions in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[52]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[53]  Signe Vangkilde,et al.  The earliest electrophysiological correlate of visual awareness? , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[54]  Sirko Straube,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of figure–ground segregation directly reflect perceptual saliency , 2010, Vision Research.

[55]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Boundary Completion Is Automatic and Dissociable from Shape Discrimination , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Mika Koivisto,et al.  The relationship between awareness and attention: Evidence from ERP responses , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  I. Rock,et al.  Perception without attention: Results of a new method , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  Dominique Lamy,et al.  Neural Correlates of Subjective Awareness and Unconscious Processing: An ERP Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  Attention modulates psychophysical and electrophysiological response to visual texture segmentation in humans , 2005, Vision Research.

[60]  Mika Koivisto,et al.  The role of unattended distractors in sustained inattentional blindness , 2007, Psychological research.

[61]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Vertex potentials evoked during auditory signal detection: Relation to decision criteria , 1973 .

[62]  Minna Lehtonen,et al.  Independence of visual awareness from the scope of attention: an electrophysiological study. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[63]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Learning to Link Visual Contours , 2008, Neuron.

[64]  A. Richards,et al.  Predicting and manipulating the incidence of inattentional blindness , 2010, Psychological research.

[65]  Daniel J. Simons,et al.  The effects of individual differences and task difficulty on inattentional blindness , 2009, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[66]  C. Frith,et al.  Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. , 1999, Science.

[67]  Pejman Sehatpour,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of human object recognition processing: An integrated high-density electrical mapping and functional imaging study of “closure” processes , 2006, NeuroImage.

[68]  Albert Costa,et al.  Manipulating inattentional blindness within and across sensory modalities , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[69]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Electrical neuroimaging based on biophysical constraints , 2004, NeuroImage.

[70]  M. Koivisto,et al.  An ERP study of change detection, change blindness, and visual awareness. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[71]  Steven B. Most,et al.  What you see is what you set: sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. , 2005, Psychological review.

[72]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Do Pictures of Faces, and Which Ones, Capture Attention in the Inattentional-Blindness Paradigm? , 2009, Perception.

[73]  A. Mack Inattentional Blindness , 2003 .

[74]  Andreas K. Kreiter,et al.  Rapid contour integration in macaque monkeys , 2005, Vision Research.

[75]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition , 2001, Vision Research.

[76]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Contour Saliency in Primary Visual Cortex , 2006, Neuron.

[77]  A. Revonsuo,et al.  Timing of the earliest ERP correlate of visual awareness. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[78]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognition performance in humans , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[79]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Illusory Contour Processing: Combined High-Density Electrical Mapping, Source Analysis, and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  Soichiro Nomura,et al.  Activation time course of responses to illusory contours and salient region: A high-density electrical mapping comparison , 2006, Brain Research.

[81]  Caspar M. Schwiedrzik,et al.  Expectations Change the Signatures and Timing of Electrophysiological Correlates of Perceptual Awareness , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[82]  Manfred Fahle,et al.  Closure facilitates contour integration , 2007, Vision Research.

[83]  Mika Koivisto,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of visual consciousness and selective attention , 2007, Neuroreport.

[84]  F. Kruggel,et al.  Hemodynamic and Electroencephalographic Responses to Illusory Figures: Recording of the Evoked Potentials during Functional MRI , 2001, NeuroImage.

[85]  Lamme Vaf,et al.  Why visual attention and awareness are different , 2003 .

[86]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[87]  K. Grill-Spector The neural basis of object perception , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[88]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The attentional requirements of consciousness , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.